Brain Rules for Aging Well. John Medina

Чтение книги онлайн.

Читать онлайн книгу Brain Rules for Aging Well - John Medina страница 11

Автор:
Серия:
Издательство:
Brain Rules for Aging Well - John Medina

Скачать книгу

too.

      One experiment involved younger people (average age twenty-four) and older people (average age seventy-three) gazing at happy and sad faces. Which would they pay the most attention to (“attentional bias”)? When the youngsters looked at positive faces, they scored 5 out of 25 on the bias scale; they scored 3 out of 25 for negative faces. This meant they were paying modest, fairly balanced attention to each. When seniors looked at the same faces, they scored 15 out of 25 for positive faces and –12 of 25 (yes, negative 12) for the not-so-positive faces. Nothing modest or equal there.

      Researchers observed similar differences when they assessed that most dissonant note of aging neurons—negative memories. To understand these data, we need briefly to review how memory works. (We’ll have a more full-throated treatment in the memory chapter.) The important concept is this: brains don’t record life as if on a single reel-to-reel tape deck. Rather, many semi-independent memory subsystems exist—many types of tape decks, if you will—each responsible for recording and retrieving a specific domain of learning. Learning how to ride a bicycle, for example, uses a different neural deck than remembering an episode of Breaking Bad or recalling that Tony Bennett sang “Put on a Happy Face.” Your ability to recognize something you’ve seen before (recognition memory) uses yet another memory subsystem.

      To test recognition memory, both younger and older populations were shown pictures of “positive images” and “negative images” (such as a person making a happy or a sad face). Younger adults recalled both at roughly equal percentages. Not so for older adults. Their recognition scores were 106 percent higher for positive images than for negative ones.

      Researchers have noticed analogous changes for episodic memory (memories for events), short-term memory (now called working memory), and long-term memory (just what it sounds like). The phenomenon even has a name: the Positivity Effect. One reason older people report being happier is that they’re increasingly selective about what they pay attention to—and what they remember when they do pay attention.

      Why all this optimism in seniors? After all, their joints begin to ache for reasons that become increasingly untreatable, friends begin dying off as if in a war zone, they forget why they went downstairs, and they quit remembering your birthday. Happiness is probably one of the many rewards the brain uses to keep us pro-social. Emphasizing positivity keeps depression away, buffering against suicide. People who are more positive toward us are more likely to lend us a hand in our old age—useful for survival.

      There’s another pro-social reason seniors are happier. To explain it, I’ll turn to an Industrial Age Brit who would not be caught dead putting on a happy face. We’re going to talk about that quintessential grumpy old man, Ebenezer Scrooge.

       Lessons from London

      The most unsettling aspect of Dickens’s A Christmas Carol to me is that some of its nineteenth-century pages seem lifted straight out of a twenty-first-century geroscience textbook. As proof, I offer you a few degrees of Ebenezer Scrooge’s famous narrative arc. He starts as a miser about Christmas, as you know, and doesn’t change to Santa Claus until he finally confronts his death. What helps turn him away from the dark side isn’t a grave marker, however. (Presumably death is as much a concern to innocent Tiny Tim as to greedy moneylenders.) The change agents come gradually, from the types of things Scrooge observes as the ghosts force-feed him his biography. When Scrooge is young, his mind is on his newly minted career, the knowledge-based world of industrial-age banking—and his increasingly self-centered success. But when he is old and the Spirits have held sway, his priorities have been turned upside down (or, rather, right side up). He exchanges the cold, knowledge-soaked world of accounts receivable for the warm, emotion-soaked earthiness of human relationships.

      Here, data and Dickens meet, for this exchange—minus the ghosts—is exactly what happens to our brains with age. We, too, shift from paying off college debt and other financial priorities to playing with grandkids. This, on average, makes us happier. The delightful metamorphosis stems from both nurture and nature, each of which deserve a hearing.

      In your youth, your brain fools you into believing you’ll live a long time, if not forever. This is an attitude positively shackled with social consequences—ranging from whether you commit to retirement savings or sign up for health care. (Insurance companies often call people in this age group “the immortals.”) You are also at the starting gate of your career, and so you see knowledge-based pursuits as your top priorities for future achievements. Ditto for your relational successes. Anyone who’s been married, had children, or experienced both understands how much extra knowledge you need to be successful.

      All that changes with age. You now have a few more miles on your biological tires and greater knowledge about how the world works. You hardly need three ghosts to realize you were wrong and won’t live forever. I remember first discovering this when I wrote down the number of books I wanted to read before kicking the bucket. I calculated the time it would take to read them and realized I would need to live more than 180 years to finish. And that’s if I didn’t do anything else but read books. While that is surely a vision of heaven for me, I have, unfortunately, other things to do. Aging forced me to prioritize. And since I knew I wanted to spend more time with my family than with Dickens, or any other author, I could sense something warmly relational shifting beneath my behavioral feet.

      This shifting is consistent with the research literature. When you truly realize you have a sell-by date, like the post-ghost Scrooge, you begin to prize relationships over just about everything else. And any time you prioritize the socioemotional components of life, you become happier—the entire point of the friendships chapter. This shift is so common, and is backed by so much empirical support, it’s been christened with its own tediously academic title: socioemotional selectivity theory.

      At the same time scientists puzzled over the weight of these behavioral data, others began ruminating over their potential neurological origins. They came up with their own, arguably more disturbing, name for their findings: FADE, short for frontal-amygdalar age-related differences in emotion.

      We’ve already discussed one of those differences: the more social relationships you acquire, the bigger the amygdala becomes. Other differences also accrue with age. Aging brains activate the appropriate emotions with greater strength—just as those emotions are changing the way we react to the world. It’s quite possible that the neurological effects termed FADE directly affect what we think is important. A lifetime of Christmas geese results.

       Roller-coaster grandpa

      Older people are supposed to be allergic to risk. Don’t tell that to Gary Coleman, a retired pastor from Ohio, however.

      Looking something like the actor Sean Penn, if Sean Penn were seventy-four, Reverend Coleman is a roller-coaster fanatic. In 2015 he took his twelve thousandth spin on Ohio’s legendary Diamondback roller coaster. “I thought it was the best coaster I’ve ridden on my whole life,” he exclaimed in an interview. “At my age, it’s great!” He knows of what he speaks. He’s been riding roller coasters obsessively since childhood.

      Researchers have found two interesting patterns concerning how risk-related behavior changes as people age, and they’re definitely related to happiness, just like the good reverend’s roller-coaster experience. One is called the “certainty effect,” the other “prevention motivation.”

      The certainty research was initially hobbled by uncertainty. That’s because young and old are willing to take risks at roughly equal rates, and with roughly equal enthusiasms. Knowing that equality doesn’t always mean similarity, however, the researchers put their heads down and charged straight at their spreadsheets. That’s when they smashed into something solid. The kinds of risks the generations take are as different as a noisy casino is from a cozy teahouse.

Скачать книгу