Practitioner's Guide to Using Research for Evidence-Informed Practice. Allen Rubin
Чтение книги онлайн.
Читать онлайн книгу Practitioner's Guide to Using Research for Evidence-Informed Practice - Allen Rubin страница 31
Typically, however, replications of experiments produce inconsistent results (as do replications of studies using other designs). Moreover, replications of studies that evaluate different interventions relevant to the same EIP question can accumulate and produce a bewildering array of disparate findings as to which intervention approach is the most effective. The studies at the top level of the hierarchy – systematic reviews (SR) and meta-analyses – attempt to synthesize and develop conclusions from the diverse studies and their disparate findings. Thyer (2004) described systematic reviews (SR) as follows:
In an SR, independent and unbiased researchers carefully search for every published and unpublished report available that deals with a particular answerable question. These reports are then critically analyzed, and – whether positive or negative, whether consistent or inconsistent – all results are assessed, as are factors such as sample size and representativeness, whether the outcome measures were valid, whether the interventions were based on replicable protocols or treatment manuals, what the magnitude of observed effects were, and so forth. (p. 173)
Although systematic reviews often will include and critically analyze every study they find, not just randomized experiments, they should give more weight to randomized experiments than to less controlled studies in developing their conclusions. Some systematic reviews, such as those registered with the Campbell or Cochrane collaborations, require researchers to meet strict standards related to methods used to find studies and quality standards for the studies that will or will not be included in the review itself.
A more statistically oriented type of systematic review is called meta-analysis. Meta-analyses often include only randomized experiments, but sometimes include quasi-experimental designs and other types of studies as well. The main focus of meta-analysis is to aggregate the statistical findings of different studies that assess the effectiveness of a particular intervention. A prime aim of meta-analysis is to calculate the average strength of an intervention's effect by aggregating the effect strength reported in each individual study. Meta-analyses also can assess the statistical significance of the aggregated results. When meta-analyses include studies that vary in terms of methodological rigor, they also can assess whether the aggregated findings differ according to the quality of the methodology. The most powerful approach to a systematic review is the combination of the rigorous and transparent searching methods, clear criteria for inclusion an exclusion of selected studies, and statistical aggregation of data.
Some meta-analyses will compare different interventions that address the same problem. For example, a meta-analysis might calculate the average strength of treatment effect across experiments that evaluate the effectiveness of exposure therapy in treating PTSD, then do the same for the effectiveness of eye movement desensitization and reprocessing (EMDR) in treating PTSD, and then compare the two results as a basis for considering which treatment has a stronger impact on PTSD.
You can find some excellent sources for unbiased systematic reviews and meta-analyses in Table 2.2 in Chapter 2. Later in this book, Chapter 8 examines how to critically appraise systematic reviews and meta-analyses. Critically appraising them is important because not all of them are unbiased or of equal quality. It is important to remember that to merit a high level on the evidentiary hierarchy, an experiment, systematic review, or meta-analysis needs to be conducted in an unbiased manner. In that connection, what we said earlier about Table 3.1 is very important, and thus merits repeating here:
This hierarchy assumes that each type of study is well designed. If not well designed, then a particular study would merit a lower level on the hierarchy.
For example, a randomized experiment with egregiously biased measurement would not deserve to be at Level 3 and perhaps would be so fatally flawed as to merit dropping to the lowest level. The same applies to a quasi-experiment with a severe vulnerability to a selectivity bias.
3.3.5 Matrix of Research Designs by Research Questions
As we have discussed, different research designs are better suited for certain types of EIP questions. As an alternative to presenting multiple research hierarchies, some researchers have represented the fit between various EIP questions and the research designs most equipped to answer those questions with a matrix to emphasize that multiple designs can be used for different types of questions, but that some designs are stronger than others depending on the question type. Table 3.2 is an example of such a matrix that we've created using the four EIP questions and the study designs that we have briefly explored in this chapter.
The larger checks indicate the study designs that are best suited to answer each EIP question, and the smaller checks indicate designs that are less well suited, or less commonly used, to answer each EIP question. Those designs without a large or small check do not provide good evidence to answer the EIP question. For example, when answering questions about factors that predict desirable and undesirable outcomes, correlational studies are the study design of choice and therefore are marked with a large check. Also, systematic reviews that combine the results of these types of studies to answer prognosis and risk questions are also marked with a large check. Sometimes the results of experimental or quasi-experimental studies are used to determine risk and prognosis as well, especially if they are large studies that collect a lot of data about the participants in order to look at factors related to risks and benefits, not just the treatments received. Therefore, each of these designs is marked with a small check. Qualitative studies and single-case designs are neither well suited to answering risk and prognosis questions nor commonly used for this purpose, so these are not marked with a check.
TABLE 3.2 Matrix of Research Designs by Research Questions
Qualitative | Experimental | Quasi-Experimental | Single Case | Correlational | Systematic Reviews or Meta-analyses | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
What factors predict desirable and undesirable outcomes? | √ | √ | √ | √ | ||
What can I learn about clients, service delivery, and targets of intervention from the experiences of others? | √ | √ | √ | |||
What assessment tools should be used? | √ |
√
|