The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Social Development. Группа авторов

Чтение книги онлайн.

Читать онлайн книгу The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Social Development - Группа авторов страница 58

The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Social Development - Группа авторов

Скачать книгу

Geary and Bjorklund have frequently collaborated, much of their work has been conducted within their own research teams. During the last 20 years Bjorklund and his team have suggested there are three types of developmental adaptations: Ontogenetic adaptations; Deferred adaptations; and Conditional adaptations.

      Looking briefly at each of these in turn, ontogenetic adaptations evolved to help the child survive at a specific point in development, but disappear when no longer required. Examples of this include neonatal imitation, milk teeth, and fear of strangers (Myers & Bjorklund, 2020; Workman & Reader, 2021). Notably, ontogenetic adaptations include various aspects of cognitive immaturity. An example of this is that children regularly overestimate their own abilities. The suggestion here is that, if children understood how poorly they actually performed on many tasks they would most likely give up at an early stage. In one study, for example, Shin et al. (2007) found first‐grade children who overestimated their performance on memory trials later showed greater improvement on memory tasks than those who did not overestimate this ability. Hence, this overestimation of ability can later lead to improvements in ability. Myers and Bjorklund (2020) suggest there may be a whole raft of such social and cognitive ontogenetic adaptations that are appropriate for that age, but which then disappear.

      Deferred adaptations are those which have no immediate benefit but are useful later in development. Social and object play are good examples of this. Many social skills are learned through engaging in play with others. Such skills are put to good use later in life in order to negotiate the social landscape. Many would suggest that infants and young children play together because it is enjoyable and leads to immediate gratification. But the ultimate reason why it is enjoyable, according to evolutionists such as Geary and Bjorklund, is because ancestral children who found it to be rewarding reaped the benefits later in life and passed on this proclivity to their offspring.

      An evolutionary stance that begins with the EEA helps us to look to the past to prepare for the present and the future. One problem that evolution helps us to understand is the ongoing dismay of parents all over the world (Barkow, 2014, Barkow et al., 2012): many ambitious adolescents equate “success” with being a successful entertainer, especially with being a rapper or rock star. Parents who for generations have been farmers or glassblowers or members of the clergy learn, at times with chagrin, of their children’s ambitions. Almost everywhere, many mothers and fathers ask the same question, “why won’t the children listen?” The answer has to do with cultural editing.

      Very briefly, this parental pain is the result of the new mass media subverting the psychological mechanisms underlying cultural editing. Cultural editing is a process without which we could not have evolved the distinctive human trait of massive cultural transmission (Barkow, 1989, 2014; Barkow et al., 2012). Our main advantage over other animal species is that, due, in part, to our ability to share attention and intention and to cooperate, we are the most cultural of the world’s species, we have evolved to adapt to our environment by socially transmitting vast encyclopedias of frequently useful information. That information is passed on within generations and across generations, but this is a very risky process: environments alter, errors occur, and exemplars die without having disseminated the knowledge they carry. And so, information that once was likely adaptive may now be lost or neutral or maladaptive or simply mistaken (Barkow, 1989). We therefore have been selected for editing mechanisms.

      The cultural editing framework leads to many questions about children’s social development. How does the learn‐preferentially‐from‐the‐high‐in‐status process change as the child grows? Is there a conflict between the evolutionary requirement that children be adapted to their current age and situation if they are to thrive or at least survive, and the need to acquire knowledge and values that will be valuable in adult life (e.g., if the local group laughs at school achievement but skill in STEM [Science, Technology, Engineering and Math] fields is key to future success)? Is some of the sturm und drang of adolescence a formally adaptive switch from perceiving parents as very high in status to perceiving other adolescents or non‐kind adults as very high in status and so learning from these new sources of cultural information? Are different categories of information processed differently at different ages? Do the cultural information editing mechanisms differ when biological sex differs? Do the different types of attachment discussed earlier in this chapter impact the operation of this cultural editing process? Are there distinct editing mechanisms that have not yet been identified? Are the mechanisms affected by family composition, health status, or by climate or subsistence economy?

      Researchers in the social development of children have chosen their field wisely: There is no shortage of future research questions.

      1 Ainsworth, M. D. (1967). Infancy in Uganda. John Hopkins University Press.

      2 Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1989). Attachments beyond infancy. American Psychologist, 44(4), 709–716.

      3 Ainsworth, M. D. S., & Bowlby, J. (1991). An ethological approach to personality development. American Psychologist, 46(4), 331–341.

      4 Auchus, R. J., & Rainey, W. E. (2004). Adrenarche–physiology, biochemistry and human disease. Clinical Endocrinology, 60(3), 288–296.

      5 Barkow, J. H. (1989). Darwin,

Скачать книгу