The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Social Development. Группа авторов

Чтение книги онлайн.

Читать онлайн книгу The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Social Development - Группа авторов страница 65

The Wiley-Blackwell Handbook of Childhood Social Development - Группа авторов

Скачать книгу

15th centuries” (Postman, 1992, p. 82); “in the new media climate everything is available to everyone at the same time: electronic media cannot keep secrets …” and “without secrets the notion of the ‘child’ is void …” (Postman, 1992, p. 83).

      It is important to realize that many books have been published in the 1970s that intend to free the child from the chains of its immaturity. An example of such books is that by Kuijer (1980). Other examples are Illich’s book, calling for a “deschooling” of society as it hinders children from participating in an adult society (Illich, 1973); Holt’s book, consistently pleading for freeing the child from the chains of a 300‐year‐old tradition of servitude (Holt, 1976); and Farson’s book, interpreting the rights of the child very literally and broadly, e.g., by demanding that children are given the right to vote, “because adults do not stand up for their interests and cannot vote on their behalf” (Farson, 1974, p. 179).

      Interestingly, the period discussed by Postman in relation to the disappearance of childhood, the 1970s, also witnessed an unprecedented large global research effort, centering on undermining Piaget’s structural cognitive theory. In other words, the non‐inter‐convertible developmental stages, referred to as cognitive structures, were gradually replaced by continuous domain‐specific developmental processes. Neo‐Piagetian research from that time undermined the presumptions of the Rousseau‐Piaget tradition, which emphasized the inaccessibility of childlike thinking, like never before. The fanaticism with which the origins of all kinds of childlike rationality were explored, caused many a researcher to end up as an “infancy expert” (Koops, 1990, 2004). This post neo‐Piagetian research, among other things, resulted in research on the Child’s Theory of Mind experimentally demonstrating how 2–3‐year‐old children already have a command of current lay psychology, based on a simple theory of desires and beliefs. Meanwhile, the search for the increasingly younger origins of generally human means of communication has not come to an end. Onishi and Baillargeon (2005), for example, demonstrated in an article in Science that 13‐month‐old babies basically have a command of generally human, ordinary communication principles (“beliefs” and “desires”). Two remarkable books summarize the surprising cognitive abilities of very young children (Wellman, 2014; Gopnik, 2009) Remarkably, cultural historical developments – the disappearance of traditional childhood – go hand in hand with the experimental empirical scientific search for (and finding of!) generally human and age‐independent means of communication. To put it briefly, developmental psychology moves with the tides of culture.

      Raising children will have to be re‐invented. We are assisted by a tremendous amount of sophisticated and splendid studies on child behavior and on that of their up‐bringers. However, I would like to point out that all this research will only prove advantageous if we know what our objective is with regard to children, and that is what we are in the dark about. Worse still, modern academic pedagogy is hardly occupied with it. People who like me, who are followers of Kant’s much maligned successor, the educationalist Johann Herbart (1776–1841), are convinced that pedagogy as a science cannot do without ethics at the one hand and (developmental) psychology on the other (Herbart, 1841). The first helps to formulate objectives, the latter offers the means to achieve them. An evolutionary view like Bjorklund’s (2007), however relevant, will not help out. Bjorklund explained why a lengthy human youth is necessary to be able to adapt to an ever‐changing culture. This view only makes clear why we should cherish an extended explorative childhood, but not if and how it should be oriented.

      Let us return for a final time to the example set by Rousseau. His incredibly effective book on education was a book on a new ethical person in a Utopian society. This very context turned his book into such a success. Of course, we are not in a position of again starting a revolutionary vision on child development. Nevertheless, we could – as Rousseau did – develop a vision of an ideal society in the spirit of which we would like to raise our children. In doing so, I recommend a restoration of the Enlightenment principles of rationality and autonomous and critical thinking; high‐grade ethical principles forming the basis of a modern “Contrat Social” (Rousseau, 1762); and commitment to a democratic society in which freedom of speech and inter‐human respect are balanced. All this needs to be worked out. However, it is good and reassuring to know that we can fall back on enlightened classical literature. But only when we have clear normative notions will we be able to profit from the rich modern empirical developmental psychology, that is today at our disposal.

      This chapter hopefully demonstrates that understanding of childhood and child development is not fully and satisfyingly possible without studying the cultural historical context. Empirical analytical research is necessary, but not sufficient. Historical framing and reframing is the key to the contextual understanding of children. In short: what we need is a Historical Developmental Psychology (Koops & Kessel, 2017).

      This chapter builds on a book of the author, published in the Dutch language (Koops, 2016).

      1 Ariès, P. (1960). L’enfant et la vie familiale sous l’ancien régime. Libraire Plon.

      2 Ariès, P. (1962). Centuries of childhood: A social history of family life. Vintage Books.

      3 Baggerman, A., & Dekker, R. (2005). Kind van de toekomst. De wondere wereld van Otto van Eck (1780–1798). Wereldbibliotheek.

      4 Baggerman, A., & Dekker, R. (2006). Verlichte pedagogiek rond 1800: ideaal, praktijk en doorwerking. De opvoeding van Otto van Eck (1780–1798). In N. Bakker, R. Dekker, & A. Janssens (Eds.), Tot burgerschap en deugd. Volksopvoeding in de negentiende

Скачать книгу