Lifespan Development. Tara L. Kuther
Чтение книги онлайн.
Читать онлайн книгу Lifespan Development - Tara L. Kuther страница 22
![Lifespan Development - Tara L. Kuther Lifespan Development - Tara L. Kuther](/cover_pre651151.jpg)
You know what a dream is?
When you are asleep and you see something
Where does it come from?
The sky
Can you see it?
No! Yes, when you’re asleep
Could I see it if I was there?
No.
Why not?
Because it is in front of us. … When you are asleep you dream and you see them, but when you aren’t asleep you don’t see them.
(Piaget, 1929, p. 93)
Open-ended interviews permit participants to explain their thoughts thoroughly and in their own words. They also enable researchers to gather a large amount of information quickly. Open-ended interviews are very flexible as well. However, their flexibility poses a challenge: When questions are phrased differently for each person, responses may not capture real differences in how people think about a given topic and instead may reflect differences in how the questions were posed and followed up by the interviewer.
A structured interview poses the same set of questions to each participant in the same way. On one hand, structured interviews are less flexible than open-ended interviews. On the other hand, because all participants receive the same set of questions, differences in responses are more likely to reflect true differences among participants and not merely differences in the manner of interviewing. For example, Evans, Milanak, Medeiros, and Ross (2002) used a structured interview to examine American children’s beliefs about magic. Children between the ages of 3 and 8 were asked the following set of questions: What is magic? Who can do magic?
Is it possible to have special powers? Who has special powers?
Does someone have to learn to do magic? Where have you seen magic? (p. 49)
After compiling and analyzing the children’s responses as well as administering several cognitive tasks, the researchers concluded that even older children, who have the ability to think logically and perform concrete operations, may display magical beliefs.
To collect data from large samples of people, scientists may compile and use questionnaires, also called surveys, made up of sets of questions, typically multiple choice. Questionnaires can be administered in person, online, or by telephone, email, or postal mail. Questionnaires are popular data collection methods because they are easy to use and enable scientists to collect information from many people quickly and inexpensively. Scientists who conduct research on sensitive topics, such as sexual interest and experience, often use questionnaires because they can easily be administered anonymously, protecting participants’ privacy. For example, the Monitoring the Future Study is an annual survey of 50,000 eighth-, tenth-, and twelfth-grade students that collects information about their behaviors, attitudes, and values concerning drug and alcohol use (Miech et al., 2017). The survey permits scientists to gather an enormous amount of data, yet its anonymity protects the adolescents from the consequences of sharing personal information that they might not otherwise reveal.
Despite their ease of use, self-report measures are not without challenges. Sometimes people give socially desirable answers: They respond in ways they would like themselves to be perceived or believe researchers desire. A college student completing a survey about cheating, for example, might sometimes look at nearby students’ papers during examinations, but she might choose survey answers that do not reflect this behavior. Her answers might instead match the person she aspires to be or the behaviors she believes the world values—that is, someone who does not cheat on exams. Self-report data, then, may not always reflect people’s true attitudes and behavior. Some argue that we are not always fully aware of our feelings and therefore cannot always provide useful insight into our own thoughts and behavior with the use of self-report measures (Newell & Shanks, 2014).
Physiological Measures
Physiological measures are increasingly used in developmental research because cognition, emotion, and behavior have physiological indicators. For example, when speaking in public, such as when you give a class presentation, do you feel your heart beat more rapidly or your palms grow sweaty? Increases in heart rate and perspiration are physiological measures of anxiety that might be measured by researchers. Other researchers might measure cortisol, a hormone triggered by the experience of stress (Simons, Cillessen, & de Weerth, 2017).
Some researchers measure eye movements or pupil dilation as indicators of attention and interest. For example, researchers who tracked participants’ eye movements as they viewed Facebook feeds learned that people are naturally attracted to social and news posts that are rich with pictures and links, yet most people are unable to report what they have viewed, even immediately after viewing it (Vraga, Bode, & Troller-Renfree, 2016). A researcher who employs physiological measures might use an infant’s pupil dilation as a measure of interest (Wetzel, Buttelmann, Schieler, & Widmann, 2016). An advantage of physiological measures is they do not rely on verbal reports and generally cannot be faked. A challenge to physiological measures is that, although physiological responses can be recorded, they may be difficult to interpret. For example, excitement and anger may both cause an increase in heart rate. Physiological measures of brain activity are a particularly promising source of data, as discussed in the Brain and Biological Influences on Development feature. Data collection methods are summarized in Table 1.6.
Research Designs
There are many steps in conducting research. In addition to determining the research question and deciding what information to collect, scientists must choose a research design—a technique for conducting the research study.
Case Study
A case study is an in-depth examination of a single person (or small group of individuals). It is conducted by gathering information from many sources, such as through observations, interviews, and conversations with family, friends, and others who know the individual. A case study may include samples or interpretations of a person’s writing, such as poetry or journal entries, artwork, and other creations. A case study provides a rich description of a person’s life and influences on his or her development. It is often employed to study individuals who have unique and unusual experiences, abilities, or disorders. Conclusions drawn from a case study may shed light on an individual’s development but may not be generalized or applied to others. Case studies can be a source of hypotheses to examine in large-scale research.
Table 1.6
Correlational Research
Are children with high self-esteem more likely to excel at school? Are older adults with more friends happier than those with few? Are college students who work part-time less likely to graduate? All of these questions can be studied with correlational research, which permits researchers to examine relations among measured characteristics, behaviors, and events. For example, in one study, scientists examined the relationship between physical