Police Power and Race Riots. Cathy Lisa Schneider

Чтение книги онлайн.

Читать онлайн книгу Police Power and Race Riots - Cathy Lisa Schneider страница 5

Автор:
Серия:
Издательство:
Police Power and Race Riots - Cathy Lisa Schneider

Скачать книгу

British scholars in the field. David Waddington developed a “Flashpoint model” listing six factors that together explained why minor altercations could spiral out of control: 1) structural—poverty, unemployment, relative deprivation, and racial discrimination; 2) political/ideological—a group’s political legitimacy, power, and influence or lack thereof; 3) cultural—the rules, norms, and self-definition a group develops and the compatibility of those norms with those of the police and society at large; 4) contextual—history of negative interactions between a minority group and the police, leading to a breakdown in communication; 5) situational—specific spatial and symbolic characteristics of the site of conflict; and 6) interactional—personal miscommunication, signaling, and misreading of particular actions.49

      Waddington’s emphasis on conflicting interpretation of events rather than the events themselves and his insistence that the precipitating event might be quite minor make specificity difficult. Waddington recognizes that structural inequality has a deleterious impact on the relationship between police and racial minorities, but he does not identify the processes or mechanisms that structure inequality along race or ethnic lines, or the ways that police actions are rooted in and perpetuate categorical inequality. Moreover studies have provided scant support for his model. In a study of police stops and seizures, Norris and his coauthors found no evidence to support the claim that black youths behave more disrespectfully toward police than do white youths.50 Police officers were two and a half times more likely to stop blacks than their presence in the population suggested, even when blacks were significantly less likely than whites to show disrespect toward the police. Police stopped whites when they were inebriated, so those stopped behaved less respectfully. They stopped blacks, in contrast, on speculative rather than evidentiary grounds. Blacks were thus far more likely to behave respectfully.51 Police actions, in other words, had nothing to do with the behavior of blacks. Rather police were biased a priori against blacks, and blacks correctly read the situations at hand.

      Racial and ethnic prejudices are embedded in police occupational culture and work, insists Holdaway. Police officers speak in racially derogatory terms even toward their own black and Asian colleagues. Black and Asian officers “must affirm rather than challenge the values of their colleagues…. Black officers find a greater measure of acceptance among colleagues when they demonstrate physical prowess when dealing with an offender.”52 Holdaway illustrates the ways in which police work is embedded in systems of inequality and how police work depends on categorical mapping. Yet he pays insufficient attention to broader political processes. As Robert Reiner points out, British researchers became too close to the police they studied. The result was that they ignored “the wider context of social, political and economic change … concentrating on funded, short scale projects, examining trees while failing to remark on the forest.”53

      French Studies of Riots and Banlieues

      The 2005 riots also sparked a wave of theorizing in France. Most explanations fell into one of two categories. One set of work was based on statistical analysis of the correlates of riots. In a broad comparative study of neighborhoods in which riots broke out and those where they did not, for instance, Hugues Lagrange observed that riots erupted where a high percentage of African immigrants, large families, and youths resided. Africans, he claims, were more likely to have larger families (sometimes although not always due to polygamy), and children from large families tended to do poorly in school (lacking basic skills) and were more likely to engage in criminal and delinquent activities and riots.54

      Fabien Jobard concurs in a series of articles drawn largely from Lagrange’s data.55 Disputing claims made by unnamed American authors, Jobard argues that the French riots were not race riots, like American or British riots, since the rioters did not attack people or stores belonging to other races or ethnic groups (although he admits there were no such stores in the banlieues) or claim to speak on behalf of a race. Instead, he argues, in his analysis of the 208 people tried in Bobigny for rioting, that while polygamous families were rare (3 percent of case files),

      [t]he average size of the families to which they belonged was typically very large: the average number of brothers and sisters was actually 4.6, while as many as one-fifth of the relevant families contained seven or more children. A family size of this kind is indicative of the presence of a large number of families from sub-Saharan Africa—a feature of French life highlighted by correlations produced by Lagrange. The geographical locations of the riots, especially in the west of France, are also closely aligned to major settlements of the new sub-Saharan migrants (and, indeed, such locations are characterized by the concentration of large families).56

      Concentrating their efforts on preventing the riots from spreading, Jobard insists, police in 2005 failed to crack down on rioters, allowing youths to remain “free to continue their activities unhindered in smaller territories surrounded by police forces.”57 In 2005, Jobard argued that there was no evidence that French police were racially biased when they stopped “a given individual or group of individuals regardless of the signs of racism that may be observed here and there.”58 In a later study, however, conducted with René Lévy in collobaration with John Lamberth and Rachel Nield from the Open Society Foundation, they found significant racial disparities in the frequency with which police stopped pedestrians. Researchers recorded police stops at several metro stations outside shopping areas downtown. They found that police stopped Arab youths 7.8 times and black youths 6.0 times more often than they did white youths.59 Jobard and Lévy were careful to point out, however, that style of dress was a better predictor of being stopped than ethnicity alone. Police responded to the study by insisting that young people coming in from the banlieues, dressed in the style of ghetto youth, were more likely to shoplift (although they provided no evidence to support this claim). Since there had been no studies to dispute (or support) the police officers’ claims, and Jobard and Lévy had studied only shopping centers, the widely publicized study did not change policy makers’ perspectives on police reform. More critically, the study underestimated the degree of racial profiling by failing to record stops in banlieues: neighborhoods virtually occupied by police, with few stores or shopping areas.

      The second set of studies was based on ethnographic research in the banlieues. The work of Didier Lapeyronnie, for instance, demonstrates both the strengths and weaknesses of this methodological approach. On the one hand, through years of work with neighborhood youths, Lapeyronnie discovered that police violence and racial discrimination caused more anger and frustration among banlieue youths than poverty or unemployment. In a 2006 article in Déviance et société he argues that riots were a form of collective action, sparked by police violence and a perception of conflict between us and them:

      This common negative experience generates an “us” which manifests itself in opposition to the police, indeed permanent conflict with them. The “we” thus negatively constituted by hostility toward the police is not devoid of significance…. In activating the “us” and forcing the change of the legitimacy of the imperative frameworks, the incident also renders the revolt both expected and legitimate…. The death of young people at the site of police interventions always elicits strong individual and collective emotion and crystallizes the feeling of “us,” victims of injustice, in opposition to a “them,” an unjust police force.60

      On the other hand, like many ethnographers, Lapeyronnie reifies space and place, projecting motives and character faults to an entire neighborhood. He exaggerates the isolation of neighborhood residents and describes their lives as if they ended at the neighborhood edge. As such, he narrows his sites on a microcosm of dysfunctional interpersonal relationships, cultures, and behaviors. Much like the works of others who have looked at the “culture of poverty,” Lapeyronnie’s study is reminiscent of the Chicago school—a view of the city as a “mosaic of little worlds which touch but do not interpenetrate,”61 where “the primary [source] of social integration and order”62 is the geographic community and poorly integrated neighborhoods foster an array of bad behaviors. In 2001 Lapeyronnie attributed

Скачать книгу