Reason: Book I. Bo Bennett

Чтение книги онлайн.

Читать онлайн книгу Reason: Book I - Bo Bennett страница 6

Автор:
Серия:
Издательство:
Reason: Book I - Bo Bennett

Скачать книгу

converging on what we might refer to as a convergence of data or a scientific consensus. According to Wikipedia, a scientific consensus is “the collective judgment, position, and opinion of the community of scientists in a particular field of study. Consensus implies general agreement, though not necessarily unanimity.” In science, specific views change frequently, and general consensuses change occasionally. Strong scientific consensuses on well-established theories rarely change but are still provisional. To the non-scientist, there isn’t much difference between a scientific view and a scientific consensus, but in the field of science, the difference is as drastic as the difference between a 6000-year-old and 13.8 billion-year-old universe.

      People (thanks to the media) often latch onto specific studies and equate those findings with science “making up its mind.” The field of science shares some of this responsibility—science communication is and has always been a problem. The media need viewers and sensationalize and exaggerate findings and conclusions when writing about a scientific discovery, often in the form of inferring a much greater level of confidence or even certainty when the findings are actually much more conservative. In the figure below, we see a blogger’s interpretation of the headline, then the actual headline from a scientific source. Notice how the blogger not only moved from a may be to an is, but also greatly embellished the findings. Another example of this can be found in a recent study on life after death.

      BhsHYUCC.pngWhen an outlier study is published, this is often presented by the media or inferred by the public as science changing its mind, when in reality it is just another of many studies conducted in an attempt to improve our collective understanding. Outliers are studies that present findings significantly different from the norm. Statistical probability alone will produce about 1 of these type of studies for every 20 conducted. Think of an outlier as a drunk celebrity that gets caught on video streaking. This outlier is far more interesting than the other studies (the sober and clothed celebrities) and has a much greater shock value. When a multitude of studies clearly converge on a set of findings or conclusion and the public is aware of this, nobody wants to hear about yet another study that confirms what they already know (that won’t get clicks). Thanks to the availability bias, the public’s perception of confidence among scientists is radically different from what it actually is.BhsHYUCC-1.png

      Due to poor science communication, the public’s view on science is more often associated with popular TV doctors and Internet websites that are pseudo-scientific, outright fraudulent, and/or offer potentially deadly advice. These “rebels” often obtain their notoriety by holding contrarian positions that play well to the public’s optimism bias, desire for instant gratification and easily digestible information. The loud voices of a few drown out the many voices sharing unpopular, complex, scientific facts.

      People tend to confuse scientific debate about fine points of a theory with debate about the theory itself. Science is a process that thrives on scholarly debate. This debate is more prevalent in complex theories, but challenges to a theory aren’t suggestive of a theory being “wrong;” they are meant to point out gaps in the theory that should be addressed.

      When we are forced to adhere to dogma, authority, or the idea that we cannot be wrong, we explain away, attempt to discredit, or ignore new information that would cause us to reevaluate our conclusions. In science, all findings are provisional and subject to revision based on new information. Using the scientific method, our conclusions may change based on updated information and new discoveries. This is the process of learning, and as it has been said, this is not a bug in science; it is a feature.

      Money and Science: Just How Skeptical Should One Be?

      Fallacious Reasoning. This default position of distrust is based on the Ad Hominem (Circumstantial) fallacy, which is when one suggests that the person who is making the argument is biased or predisposed to take a particular stance, and therefore, the argument is necessarily invalid. More specifically, in this case, it is suggesting that research necessarily cannot be trusted because the scientist has an interest in the outcome. This is like rejecting your doctor’s advice because they are paid to take a personal interest in your health. Like your doctor’s interest in your health, the vast majority of professionals, scientists included, are interested in both their own well-being as well as the well-being of the people they serve. This doesn’t make them evil; it makes them human.

      The Demonization of “Big” Industry. Growing up, we had an arcade-style pinball machine in our basement that I used to play all the time. The game allowed you to “nudge” the ball by modestly shaking the machine, but would trigger a loud buzz and result in the end of the play for that ball if the nudge was more of a tilt. In the field of science, especially where big industry is involved, ulterior motives can and do result in both nudges and tilts. The nudges might be seen in overreaching conclusions, loose interpretations, over-hyped press releases, and other means of salesmanship that deviate from the scientific method and manage to make it past the journal editors. The tilts are statistically rare and are found mostly in the falsification of data. The misinformation introduced by these tilts don’t go unnoticed thanks to the scientific method. Given that scientific truths are arrived at by the convergence of data (i.e., multiple studies that support a general conclusion), nudges have relatively little effect—especially if research on an issue is conducted equally by those with financial interests in opposite outcomes. Whether a billion dollars is spent on a coin flipping study, or a few thousand dollars, taking into consideration the nudges and the tilts, the results that heads comes up about 50% of the time will not change. Scientific facts cannot be purchased, nor can they be bribed to go away.

      Money is Not the Greatest Motivator. There are far greater motivators than money including power, fame, prestige, meaning or purpose, being a hero by exposing perceived injustice, or believing that one is securing one’s place in Heaven by saving souls from an eternity of torture. If we are going to consider how money can influence science, we need to also consider the motivations of those who oppose science and scientific consensus. When this idea is critically evaluated, we find biases and personal interests everywhere. The difference is, the scientific method is the only method designed to deal with these biases, and it’s not bloggers, the media, political commentators, politicians, or religious leaders who deserve our trust to answer scientific questions; it’s scientists.

      It is fallacious to assume dishonesty solely because of personal interest. It is fallacious to reason that industries with the most money determine scientific facts or that the “little guy” must have the scientific truth on his side because of big bad industry. It is lazy thinking to focus on how personal interest in the form of money might influence scientific findings without considering how money along with other forms of personal interest fuel the anti-science movement. Questioning the legitimacy of a study is an important part of the scientific process, just make sure you avoid the fallacies mentioned when forming your opinion.

      1 Benson, H., Dusek, J. A., Sherwood, J. B., Lam, P., Bethea, C. F., Carpenter, W., … Hibberd, P. L. (2006). Study of the Therapeutic

Скачать книгу