Introduction to Experimental Linguistics. Sandrine Zufferey

Чтение книги онлайн.

Читать онлайн книгу Introduction to Experimental Linguistics - Sandrine Zufferey страница 9

Introduction to Experimental Linguistics - Sandrine Zufferey

Скачать книгу

impossible to measure reading comprehension in only one way or with one type of experiment. We need to narrow down this notion to a more precise variable, corresponding to a process involved in reading comprehension that can be measured. For example, this could be the elements included in the readers’ representation of the text and stored in memory once the reading has finished. One way to assess comprehension would be to ask questions about the text at the end of reading and count the number of correct answers.

      Let us look at a few more examples to understand what a research hypothesis is:

      1 (1) Bilinguals have different cognitive abilities from monolinguals.

      2 (2) Reading and understanding a text is difficult for children.

      The above-mentioned hypotheses cannot be the basis of experimental research since they do not meet the criteria listed above. Their terms are too vague, they specify neither the cause nor the effect, and do not specify any measure to rely on so as to draw conclusions.

      In order to be tested empirically, these hypotheses could be transformed into (3) and (4):

      1 (3) Bilinguals perform better than monolinguals at a cognitive flexibility task.

      2 (4) When reading a text, 10–12-year-old children draw fewer inferences than 14–16-year-old teenagers.

      In summary, a research hypothesis is based on existing knowledge in order to establish a relationship between two or more variables. It must also be operationalized, that is, clearly defining the measures that will be used for quantifying the variables being examined to verify the hypothesis.

      The construction of a good research hypothesis is the result of different stages, among which the most important are conceptualizing the hypothesis, on the basis of knowledge acquired in the field, and then operationalizing the hypothesis. We will discuss the specific stages for conceptualizing a hypothesis in Chapter 6, which is devoted to the practical aspects of an experiment. We will discuss the stages involved in the operationalization of a hypothesis in Chapter 2.

      1.2.2. Manipulation of variables

      Let us now go back to the example of the influence of working memory on reading comprehension. In this example, the variable verbal working memory can be observed in two ways. The first possibility would be to measure the skills of the people taking part in the experiment by using a verbal working memory test. According to this evaluation and its results, participants could be sorted into groups. By doing so, every participant is included under a variable modality (e.g. high competence or low competence) depending on his/her own characteristics, as some people have better working memory capacities than others. In this case, the variable is simply observed during research.

      In general, in experimental research, the aim is to manipulate all the variables involved in the hypotheses. However, due to practical or ethical reasons, this is not always possible. For example, age, socio-economic level, bilingualism, etc., cannot be manipulated because they are inherent in people. When variables can be manipulated, the decision to manipulate them, as well as the way in which to manipulate them, must follow ethical principles, ensuring that research will not harm the participants during the test. The cost/benefit relationship must be clearly considered when pondering the possibility of manipulating a variable or not. For example, imagine that you formulate a hypothesis stating that in stressful situations, people tend to speak faster than in non-stressful situations. In order to study the influence of stress on articulation rate, you could decide to manipulate the participant’s stress level. To set up a stressful condition, you could imagine putting some of the participants in a dark room in front of an audience booing at them. In experimental terms, such manipulation would be adequate, in the sense that a high level of stress would most likely result from your manipulation. On the other hand, it would be totally inappropriate from an ethical point of view. Actually, this type of manipulation would affect the participants to a much larger extent than needed, and they would probably not leave the experiment unscathed. Although this is an extreme example, it illustrates the fact that an experiment should not leave an impact trace on the participants once the experiment is over. We will develop this point in Chapter 6, which is devoted to the practical aspects of an experiment.

      1.2.3. Control of external variables

      Going back to the example of the influence of memory on reading comprehension, we may assume that educational level, general cognitive abilities, age, reading habits, etc., can influence both memory and reading comprehension. Likewise, the characteristics of the material used in the experiment may have an influence on the results. If, in the above-mentioned example, we use very simple text and questions, it is possible that everyone answers the questions perfectly well, regardless of their memory skills. On the contrary, if the text and the questions are very complicated, it is possible that very few people will be capable of answering. In these cases, we risk not finding a connection between memory and reading comprehension, not because the link doesn’t exist, but because the material used for the experiment is not suitable for evidencing such a link.

      1.2.4. The notions of participants and items

      To attenuate these potential problems, and to reduce the importance of the characteristics of the participants or the material employed, experimental research is based on data collected from a large number of people, using a broad palette of materials. Referring back to our example, it

Скачать книгу