Attachment Theory and Research. Группа авторов

Чтение книги онлайн.

Читать онлайн книгу Attachment Theory and Research - Группа авторов страница 26

Attachment Theory and Research - Группа авторов

Скачать книгу

For this abridged version a critical examination of psycho‐analytical theories relating to separation anxiety has been omitted. It is being published as a separate paper: Bowlby, J. (1960). Separation anxiety: A critical review of the literature. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 1 (4), 251–269.

      8 8 In an earlier paper (Bowlby 1957) I have used the term ‘instinctual response’ to refer both to the behaviour and to the hypothetical internal structure which, when activated, is presumed to lead to the behaviour. To avoid this confusion I am now using the term ‘instinctual response system’ for the hypothetical internal structure, and limiting the term ‘instinctual response’ to the active behavioural response, including both the motor behaviour pattern and its physiological and psychological concomitants.

      9 9 A similar view, though coupled with a materially different theory of instinct, was advanced by McDougall (1923).

      10 10 The possibility that a single emotion, fright, may accompany more than one instinctual response system suggests that Lorenz’s hypothesis that each response is accompanied by an emotional state peculiar to itself may need modification.

      11 11 See the discussion by Thorpe (1950, p. 390). Several workers (e.g. Hinde 1954a) have shown that, paradoxically, strangeness evokes both escape and curiosity, and that there is a complex balance between the two competing response systems.

      12 12 Such situations have been termed ‘consummatory situations’ by Bastock, Morris and Moynihan (1953) and by Hinde (1954b). In my view, however, partly because of the usefulness of the verb ‘to terminate’, a preferable term is ‘terminating situation’.

      13 13 ‘The tendency for instinctual responses to be directed towards a particular individual or group of individuals and not promiscuously towards many’ (Bowlby (1958, p. 370).

      14 14 The term ‘haven of safety’ has been introduced by Harlow and Zimmermann (1958). In describing their very interesting experiments with rhesus monkeys they write: ‘One function of the real mother, human or sub‐human, and presumably of a mother surrogate, is to provide a haven of safety for the infant in times of fear or danger.’ See also Harlow (1958).

      15 15 The secondary drive theory, which they invoke to account for the child’s tie to his mother and for separation anxiety, is not necessary to an interpretation of their data.

      16 16 In his experiments with goat kids Liddell (1950) has demonstrated the very different responses to fright‐evoking stimuli of identical twins according to whether or not they were with their mother: the twin with his mother roamed about naturally and seemed relaxed, whereas the one without his mother froze almost immobile in a corner of the room.

      17 17 Of Schaffer’s twenty‐five subjects, sixteen were aged over 28 weeks and nine 28 weeks and under. Of the two younger infants who deviated from the usual behaviour, one was already 28 weeks of age, and so on the margin of the older age group, and the other was thought to be missing his dummy.

      18 18 Anthony writes: ‘It would also follow that before the seventh month the infant cannot be separated from an object‐mother firmly and substantially localized in space, as an organized reality. His separation feeling must therefore lack the quality of separations at a later stage.’

      19 19 See, for instance, Winnicott’s (1953) conception of the development of the false self and Balint’s (1952) of conditions which give rise to neurosis and the need for a ‘new beginning’.

      20 20 Analytic literature is full of references to hatred arising in such situations. That separation from mother itself provokes it has been used by Freud (1920, p. 16) as a possible explanation of the cotton‐reel incident: ‘Throwing away the object so that it was “gone”, ‘he suggests, ‘might satisfy an impulse of the child’s … to revenge himself on his mother for going away from him’. Dorothy Burlingham and Anna Freud (1944), Spitz (1953), Robertson (1953a, 1953b) and Heinicke (1956) have all reported first‐hand observations of intensely hostile behaviour following separation. It is Fairbairn’s ([1951] 1952) view that the origin of the infant’s aggression towards his libidinal object, and therefore of his ambivalence, lies in the trauma of separation from mother and the consequent libidinal deprivation and frustration.

      21 21 There is another situation which may lead a child to become excessively clinging and which may also masquerade as “spoiling.” It is when a mother, for unconscious reasons of her own, communicates to a child her desire that he should not leave her. This is a common finding in cases of so‐called school phobia (Johnson et al. 1941).

      22 22 In a comparison of twenty 6‐year‐old children reported as overdependent with twenty controls, Stendler (1954) found that six of the over‐dependent children were ‘over‐protected’ and eleven had suffered major disturbances in their lives between the ages of 9 months and 3 years.

      John Bowlby

       From the John Bowlby Archive, Wellcome Collections London, PP/Bow/H10

      This paper was transcribed by Tommie Forslund and Robbie Duschinsky from John Bowlby’s notes. Some of the text had already been written on a typewriter by Bowlby, but most was in the form of handwritten notes, with additions, deletions, rewordings, and various footnotes suggesting rounds of reworking the text.

      The thesis of this essay is that, in order to understand the conditions that elicit anxiety and fear, it is necessary first to consider basic biological principles. The principles concern (a) the nature of the states held relatively steady by a living organism (categories of homeostasis), and (b) the nature of the stable pathways along which development usually proceeds (categories of homeorhesis). Anxiety and fear are experienced, it is suggested, when some form of instability is appraised as being present or imminent and, especially, when instability is appraised as increasing and difficult to correct, or when risk of instability is appraised as increasing and difficult to reduce.

      These appraisal processes, like any others, may or may not reach a phase of being felt. Because traditionally the words ‘anxiety’ and ‘fear’ refer to something felt, it is thought wisest to restrict their use to occasions when appraisal processes actually reach the phase of being felt. The term ‘unconscious anxiety’, though somewhat contradictory, is of course in common use. If used, it would apply to those same appraisal processes when they are in a phase of not being felt.

      In order to survive

Скачать книгу