Transfusion Medicine. Jeffrey McCullough

Чтение книги онлайн.

Читать онлайн книгу Transfusion Medicine - Jeffrey McCullough страница 26

Transfusion Medicine - Jeffrey McCullough

Скачать книгу

less likely to engage in behavior that puts them at risk for transfusion‐transmissible diseases, although about 1.5% report some kind of risk behavior [19]. In one study, 8% of young “potential” donors tested positive for drugs of abuse [20], but because these were not actual donors, it is not clear whether this experience would apply to blood donors.

      As the prevalence of tattooing and other body modifications has increased, a study in the Netherlands by Prinsze et al. [21] reviewed the risks of transfusion‐transmitted infection after tattooing and other needle‐related events. These donors (if deferred) often show low return rates to donation. The authors also demonstrated that donors who reported a tattoo, body piercing, or acupuncture showed no higher risks for transfusion‐transmitted infections [21].

      Psychosocial theories applicable to blood donation

      Piliavin and Callero [4] discuss five psychosocial theories that might apply to blood donor motivation: (a) Becker’s model of commitment, (b) the opponent process theory, (c) the attribution/self‐perception framework, (d) the identity theory, and (e) the theory of reasoned action. These can be described briefly as summarized from Piliavin and Callero [4].

      In Becker’s theory of commitment, the action or decision is based on background factors or preconceptions. These factors influence whether the person takes the initial action and then the person is subsequently influenced by the first few experiences. In the opponent process theory, the stronger the negative feelings before the action, the stronger are the positive feelings after successfully completing the action. Thus, despite initial fears or negative feelings, a good experience with donation could lead to a strongly positive attitude about continued donation. This theory attempts to account for the continuation of activities that were initially associated with negative feelings. The attribution theory postulates that if an individual believes that there is an external reason for the action, the action is attributed to that external force. In the identity theory, the sense of self is developed from the variety of social roles in which the individual engages. In the theory of reasoned action, the critical factor leading to an act is the development of an intention to carry out the act.

      Integrated model

      Experienced donors increasingly internalize the expectations and the role of being a blood donor. Donor identity correlates with the donation intention. Self‐determination theory indicates that people persist with behaviors that are internally versus externally motivated. This theory of self‐determination has been at the basis of thinking about blood donor recruitment [22].

      Theory of planned behavior

      This model is based on attitude, subjective norm, and perceived behavioral control [23]. These influence the intention to carry out behaviors. Although this model has a different name than those of Piliavin and Callero [4], theory of planned behavior really involves attitudes about the behavior, social pressure about the behavior, and control over performing the behavior. Thus, it is not surprising that several studies (reviewed by Masser et al. [23]) indicate that theory of planned behavior predicts a substantial portion of blood donation intention and behavior [24].

      Giving and not giving

      The reasons for donating are summarized generally [4] as: (a) extrinsic rewards and incentives, (b) intrinsic rewards and incentives, (c) perceived community needs, (d) perceived community support, (e) social pressure, and (f) addiction to donation. The reasons for not donating include: (a) medical ineligibility, (b) fear, (c) reactions and deferral (poor experiences), and (d) inconvenience and time requirements. Oswalt [6], in reviewing 60 English‐language reports regarding motivation for blood donation, concluded that the following factors were motivations to donate: (a) altruism and humanitarianism, (b) personal or family credit, (c) social pressure, (d) replacement, and (e) reward. Reasons for not donating included: (a) fear, (b) medical excuses, (c) reactions, (d) apathy, and (e) inconvenience. Rados [24] also found that fear, inconvenience, and never being asked were the most common reasons given for not donating. In general, the issues described earlier have seemed to appear rather consistently in these and other studies [25] of donor motivation or nondonation. Because they have been consistent over time, most recruitment strategies attempt to take these factors into consideration. Most blood donors have a rationale way of thinking about blood donation, but some think and make decisions about blood donation based on emotional, personal, or stereotype manner in continuing to donate blood, and the convenience of donation is a stronger factor than helping others [26]. In general, donors give blood out of altruism and in response to a general appeal or a specific request.

      About 70–80% of donors are repeat donors, although this percentage is decreasing [16, 30]. Repeat donors tend to be 16 and 17 years or older than 50 years, male, Rh negative, type O, without a reaction during donation, and have a college degree [16, 31]. A shorter interval between the first two donations also predicts more continued donations [28]. Over time a “blood donor role” develops in repeat donors, and this strengthens self‐commitment to blood donation, including “friendships contingent on donating, a self‐description as a regular donor, an increase in the ranking of the blood donor role, greater expectations from others, and even more donations” [4].

      Donors who are deferred are less likely to return to donate after the reason for the temporary deferral has passed [4, 16]. This is not surprising because deferral breaks the good feelings that might have developed about donation and makes future donation more difficult. Experiencing a reaction also reduces the likelihood of a donor returning [4, 14, 15]. This is because the donor begins to see himself or herself as someone who has trouble donating, and the reaction experience modifies any previous positive feelings about donation. Surprisingly, most multigallon donors report that they do not receive recognition for their donation, and the knowledge that a friend or relative was a blood donor did not make them more likely to donate [9]. These observations are consistent with the general view that the initial donation is motivated primarily by external factors and continued donation primarily by internal factors [32].

      A disconnect exists between blood supply shortages and demographic reports that there are actually

Скачать книгу