The Handbook of Solitude. Группа авторов

Чтение книги онлайн.

Читать онлайн книгу The Handbook of Solitude - Группа авторов страница 37

The Handbook of Solitude - Группа авторов

Скачать книгу

their offspring in these communal nests were observed to display higher levels of sensitive caregiving such as nursing and licking and grooming behaviors during the postpartum period compared to mothers who reared their offspring alone (see Curley & Champagne, 2016). Compared to offspring reared in standard conditions, offspring reared in communal nests were shown to have more positive outcomes including increased exploratory behaviors, reduced anxiety‐like behaviors, more social behaviors, and increased hippocampal and hypothalamic nerve growth factor (see Curley & Champagne, 2016). The enhanced developmental outcomes of communally reared offspring are likely a function of receiving increased responsive caregiving from mothers and other adults.

       The role of temperament in rodent social behaviors and social isolation.

      Rodents’ temperament, particularly behavioral inhibition, has been linked with the extent to which they spend their time socially with others and health‐related outcomes. Cavigelli and her colleagues (2009) assessed rats’ behavioral inhibition based on their latency to approach to novelty in both social and nonsocial situations. In social situations, rats were introduced to a novel rat placed in a wired cage; in nonsocial situations, they were introduced to novel objects. They defined behavioral inhibition as longer than median latency to approach to novelty in social and nonsocial situations. Based on this definition, 30% of any tested group showed behavioral inhibition in both situations. Notably, rats’ responses to social situations and nonsocial situations were unrelated: rats could be slow to approach novel social situations but not slow at approaching novel objects. Latency to approach an object in the nonsocial condition was relatively stable across four months, whereas latency to approach an unfamiliar rat in the social condition was not stable over the same time period. Similar to findings in humans, about 17% of rats demonstrated a stable pattern of behavioral inhibition in social situations assessed four months apart, and these inhibited rats continued to show behavioral inhibition at a third time point (for a review see Cavigelli, 2018).

      There is some evidence suggesting that behavioral inhibition leads to shorter longevity by altering the functioning of the neuroendocrine system. Behaviorally inhibited rats were shown to have 20–30% more basal glucocorticoids than noninhibited rats (Cavigelli et al., 2009), and greater levels of basal glucocorticoids in young adulthood predicted shorter life spans (Cavigelli et al., 2009). Notably, inhibition during social situations was a better predictor of basal glucocorticoid production and glucocorticoid reactivity than inhibition during nonsocial situations, suggesting that wariness in social situations may play an especially important role in altering the basal functioning of the neuroendocrine system. Behavioral inhibition was also associated with poorer cardiovascular system functioning (higher heart rate and blood pressure), which may be another mechanism by which behavioral inhibition may be linked with shorter life span (see Cavigelli, 2018). Finally, behavioral inhibition was associated with poor immune system functioning (an accentuated inflammatory response), which may also explain why this temperament trait may lead to shorter life span (see Cavigelli, 2018).

      Although behavioral inhibition in infancy has been linked with greater basal glucocorticoid production and behavioral inhibition in later life, social experiences in adolescence were shown to moderate these associations (Caruso et al., 2014). Specifically, behaviorally inhibited rats that were housed with novel social partners in adolescence showed less exploratory behavior in adulthood compared to those housed with familiar social partners. On the other hand, behaviorally noninhibited rats that were housed with novel social partners in adolescence showed lower increase in basal glucocorticoid production and increased exploration in adulthood compared to rats that were housed with familiar rats. As such, rats that were noninhibited as infants displayed behaviors and physiology linked with behavioral inhibition. These findings suggested that relatively short‐term social experiences in adolescence may lead to changes in the stability of temperament as well as glucocorticoid production.

      As reviewed in this chapter, nonhuman animal research has not only produced empirical findings that guide our understanding of how social deprivation and isolation impact developmental outcomes, but also played an important role in the formation of theories such as Bowlby’s attachment theory that have guided a fruitful body of research with humans. Although this chapter has primarily focused on nonhuman animal research, particularly Rhesus macaque. and rodents, there is also an extensive body of work examining the impact of social deprivation and social isolation in humans. For example, one form of social deprivation that is experienced by some children is growing up in institutional settings such as in orphanages. In such settings, children are often deprived of consistent and emotionally responsive caregivers due to factors such as high staff turn overs and rotations, high child to caregiver staff ratios, and insufficient staff training (Smyke et al., 2007). This form of social deprivation has been shown to have long‐lasting negative impact on children’s brain, social, and cognitive development (Hostinar et al., 2012; van Ijzendoorn et al., 2011). These findings are largely consistent with the nonhuman animal research findings reviewed in this chapter.

      Although there is a good amount of human work examining the impact of institutional care or maternal deprivation on child outcomes, few studies have examined whether the impact of social deprivation or recovery from social deprivation depends on the child’s sex. Given the evidence from rodent work suggesting that there may be sex differences in whether or how well rodents recover from social deprivation after resocialization (Arakawa, 2007), it would be important to understand whether the impact of social deprivation or children’s recovery

Скачать книгу