Cognitive Flexibility. Evelyne Clement

Чтение книги онлайн.

Читать онлайн книгу Cognitive Flexibility - Evelyne Clement страница 7

Cognitive Flexibility - Evelyne Clement

Скачать книгу

      1.2.2. Associated pathologies

      Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is often emblematic of pathologies that may be associated with an executive deficit (Willcutt et al. 2005). Recent research has demonstrated the usefulness of cognitive tests, including attention and executive measures, such as flexibility and inhibition to predict the diagnosis of ADHD in children aged 8–15 (Perrault et al. 2019).

      Cognitive flexibility difficulties have also been shown in children with pediatric bipolar disorder (Passarotti et al. 2016). Patients with bipolar disorder have a deficit in cognitive flexibility (O’Donnell et al. 2017).

      People with autism spectrum disorders also have flexibility difficulties (Hughes et al. 1994; Reed 2018). Zelazo et al. (2002) showed the possibility of predicting theory of mind difficulties in individuals with autism by their performance in cognitive flexibility.

      A deficit in cognitive flexibility has also been demonstrated in depression (Gabrys et al. 2018). Many neurodegenerative diseases can be affected by a cognitive flexibility deficit, with the two most iconic being Alzheimer’s disease (Swanberg et al. 2004) and Parkinson’s disease (Lange et al. 2018).

      A measure of cognitive flexibility can therefore be useful in the diagnosis of certain pathologies, whether in children or adults.

      Can we really talk about measuring flexibility? What are we really measuring?

      As with all executive functions, the measurement of pure flexibility seems unattainable. Regardless of the type of assessment chosen, there is no 100% guarantee that no other cognitive process is involved in a situation that is supposed to measure flexibility.

      1.3.1. The different types of assessment

      Many criteria could be used to classify the different types of flexibility assessment. The population concerned, the more or less ecological nature of the situation used, whether it is spontaneous or reactive flexibility (Eslinger and Grattan 1993; see also Chapter 5), etc., are all criteria that could allow such a classification. In this section, we have chosen to focus on the distinction based on the type of measurement.

      In the context of flexibility measurement, as in many other assessments, two main types of measurements can be used: direct measurements and indirect measurements. Measures that can be called “direct” correspond to all situations that involve an assessment of flexibility capacities through a concrete situation (a task constructed in the laboratory, in general, or a more ecological situation).

      We will present the different types of direct tasks in a first part, without claiming to be exhaustive. We will see that within these tasks, different subtypes can be identified. The second type of measurement can be called “indirect”, as it is carried out by questionnaire. The questionnaire can either be filled in by the person themselves, in the case of self-assessments, or by a third party (a relative or a carer for example), in the case of hetero-assessments.

      Both types of measurement have their advantages and disadvantages. Direct measurements allow an individual measurement at a given time of the individual’s flexibility; this measurement is precise and does not involve the subjectivity of any third party. Therefore, they seem more reliable and representative than indirect measurements, but they have the disadvantage of being more time consuming, and of not being able to be offered to all individuals (for example, very young children, very old people, people with certain pathologies or disabilities).

      1.3.1.1. Direct evaluations

      1.3.1.1.1. Historical and classical tasks

      One of the best known measurements of flexibility, or at least the most widely used and cited in this field, is the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST; Grant and Berg (1948)). This task consists of sorting cards, in which no explicit instructions are given and the sorting rule must be inferred on the basis of feedback (correct sorting or not). Once the rule is found, a rule change is performed and the task involves discovering the new sorting rule. The sorting rules are based on perceptual criteria of the elements present on the cards such as color, shape or number. As Miyake (2000) pointed out, the WCST seems to be much more than a test of cognitive flexibility and, according to him, corresponds to a test of high-level executive function, certainly involving cognitive flexibility, but not only this. This task has been considered by different researchers as an inhibition task, a problem-solving task, a categorization task, or other.

      Based on the same principle of switching from one process to another on a series of items, several tasks have been proposed that attempt to target an assessment of cognitive flexibility more directly. One of these, often considered a WCST for children, was originally proposed as a flexibility task for preschoolers: the Dimensional Change Card Sort (DCCS) by Frye et al. (1995) – for a recent meta-analysis on this task, see Doebel and Zelazo (2015). In this test, the child must sort two types of test cards (red boats and blue rabbits), matching them to two target cards (a blue boat and a red rabbit). The test consists of two blocks. In the first block, the child must sort the test cards along one dimension (color or shape) and in the second block, the child must sort the same test cards along the other dimension. The instruction is repeated at each trial and the change of dimension is explicitly announced.

      The common feature of the above tests is that they require flexible switching from one rule to another on a set of items. Other tasks, on the other hand, require a change in stimulus processing, not between several blocks of items, but from one item to another. One example is the “plus minus” task (Jersild 1927), which compares the performance obtained when performing simple operations (addition or subtraction) without the need to switch to those obtained when performing a series of operations involving a systematic switch between these two simple operations.

      This procedure can be seen as the basis of the task switching paradigm, which is used in a great deal

Скачать книгу