Animal Behavior for Shelter Veterinarians and Staff. Группа авторов

Чтение книги онлайн.

Читать онлайн книгу Animal Behavior for Shelter Veterinarians and Staff - Группа авторов страница 21

Animal Behavior for Shelter Veterinarians and Staff - Группа авторов

Скачать книгу

inside. Dogs were more likely to retreat when they heard the “my bone” growl than when they heard the “threatening stranger” growl. People may have more difficulty than dogs in evaluating growls, and people should attend to both dog behavior and context to infer meaning (Faragó et al. 2017).

      1.4.3 Olfactory Communication

      Dogs are known for their noses and with good reason. Compared to microsmatic, or “poor smelling,” animals like humans, dogs have physiological structures that prioritize smelling and can detect and discriminate a large number of, what are for humans, imperceptible odors (Horowitz 2009b). Scent particles enter the dog’s nose both by sniffing and regular breathing (Neuhaus 1981). These particles then enter the nasal cavity, where a mucus lining covers the olfactory epithelium and mediates olfaction—smelling (Furton and Myers 2001). Considerably more genes code for olfactory receptors in dogs than in humans (Quignon et al. 2003).

      Compared to humans, dogs seek out and access a much wider set of contextual and social information through smell, and olfaction is a major part of dog intra‐ and interspecific social encounters. Dogs, like many mammals, have a secondary molecule‐detection organ, the vomeronasal organ (VNO), that is directly involved in social communication and pheromone assessment (Adams and Wiekamp 1984). Distinct from the main olfactory epithelium, the VNO is located below the nasal cavity, and its receptors also carry information to the olfactory bulb. This chemosensory organ is ordinarily viewed as responsible for pheromone detection in urine, feces, and saliva as well as glands in the anogenital region, mouth, and face. Using odor cues, dogs can discriminate conspecifics as well as identify something unique about themselves compared to other odors (Bekoff 2001; Horowitz 2017). Horowitz (2020) also found that dogs can distinguish their owner’s odor from that of a stranger. Additionally, dogs appear to take note of human odors associated with fear or happiness (D’Aniello et al. 2018).

      Dog social encounters are marked by close olfactory inspection, particularly of the head and anogenital area. Body sniffing is common between dogs when they first meet, either on or off leash (Bradshaw and Lea 1992; Westgarth et al. 2010). Initial encounters are typically short, and dogs often explore the environment instead of furthering the interaction, a phenomenon that has been described in free‐ranging dogs and at dog parks (Howse et al. 2018; Ward 2020). In the samples studied, often self‐selected groups at dog parks or open spaces, post‐greeting aggression and even play were relatively rare. Direct encounters between dogs at shelters may be rare, and dogs tend to be on leash (or in kennels), and interactions might be thwarted due to shelter operational protocols. Without the opportunity for direct olfactory investigation, these dogs might experience tension, restraint, or frustration upon seeing other dogs, which could affect subsequent intraspecific interactions.

      Communication via scents is common by depositing secretions and excretions in the environment. Urination is more than waste expulsion; canids gain valuable social information by attending to these splatterings and pay considerable attention to unfamiliar urine (Lisberg and Snowdon 2009). Scent marking can even be performed differently between dogs of the same sex. Small male dogs have been found to mark higher than they are tall; hypotheses for why they do this have not yet been tested (McGuire et al. 2018). Depending on the surface, scent marks could be visual, olfactory, or even, possibly, auditory (Bekoff 1979).

      Olfaction is essential to the dog umwelt, or perceptual world (Horowitz 2009b). Providing dogs with opportunities to actively use their noses—for instance, through nosework—can enhance well‐being. Duranton and Horowitz (2019) found that dogs who participated in nosework for two weeks displayed a more optimistic outlook—measured via cognitive bias test—when compared to dogs who participated in heelwork practice. Engaging in sniffing is good for dogs. Humans’ jobs, as their observers and caretakers, is to know that the dog's nose is constantly engaged, to actively provide them with smelling opportunities, and to not discourage them from using their nose in species‐typical ways.

      Behavioral and physiological parameters help assess canine negative and positive affective states. This section focuses on the former, particularly multi‐modal behavior patterns relevant for dog welfare and well‐being both in and out of the shelter.

      Despite the good intentions of people and organizations, shelters are awash in stressors. While adaptive in that the stress response and fear prompt physiological and behavioral changes when something is perceived as frightening or indicative of danger (Boissy 1995), stressors (or the perception of such) can also be unrelenting. Persistent or chronic stress challenge short‐ and long‐term well‐being, and studies identify relationships between stress and a decrease in immune function (Glaser and Kiecolt‐Glaser 2005), a decrease in lifespan (Dreschel 2010), and an increase in arousal, fear, and aggression (Dreschel and Granger 2005).

      Stress‐related behaviors summarized by Mariti et al. (2012) overlap with fear, anxiety, conflict, or appeasement. Snout/lip licking, yawning, whining, and panting are oral behaviors. Dogs may avoid eye contact, look away, or have their ears back. Trembling and body shaking are often indicators of high psychological stress and could be accompanied by a lowered body posture, cowering, and hiding. Dogs paw‐lift in both social and asocial contexts, when alone and distressed, and also during social (inter‐ or intraspecific) conflict, confusion, or fear (e.g., of punishment). Periods of continual barking, whining, and howling suggest frustration or distress, although vocalization could also be socially mediated and serve other functions. Dogs with either low or high activity should be monitored for additional behaviors of anxiety. Dogs may also be excessive or under‐consumers of food and water. Individual differences are common, and behaviors presented can be affected by stressor type, such as social or non‐social context, as well as severity and duration. Overt indicators such as trembling and whining may be more recognizable than subtle behaviors, for example, turning away, yawning, and licking (Mariti et al. 2012).

      Dog bites to people can occur in any context where dogs and humans overlap, such as situations where resources are present (like food or toys), on private property, in play, and during seemingly “normal” interactions (like petting or being in a dog’s presence), as well as in occupational contexts such as those involving entering the dog’s home environment, or veterinary or shelter work, among others. People are often familiar with the dog who bit them, and young children, who are often bitten on the face or upper body, constitute a large number of those bitten (Reisner et al. 2011). Breed, neuter status, age, and sex are often explored as contributory factors to bites, yet relationships between these factors and bites are not always clear and should be interpreted cautiously (Newman et al. 2017). Instead, the conditions under which a dog is reared, kept, or managed—such as lacking socialization experience or being left unsupervised with children—could increase bite risk. A UK survey found that numerous factors influence the presence of aggressive displays, and a dog who shows aggressive behavior in one context, such as outside

Скачать книгу