Understanding Clinical Papers. David Bowers

Чтение книги онлайн.

Читать онлайн книгу Understanding Clinical Papers - David Bowers страница 19

Understanding Clinical Papers - David  Bowers

Скачать книгу

consumption. Suppose also that you found the corollary – that low mortality areas were associated with low tobacco consumption – then your findings would support a link between the supposed risk (smoking) and the target disorder (lung cancer).

Schematic illustration of the findings from an ecological study about smoking and domestic fires.

      Source: From Diekman et al. (2008), © 2008, BMJ Publishing Group Ltd.

      But you may by now have spotted a flaw in this type of study: we don't know whether the individual people who died in house fires were smokers. Put another way, it is possible for a study of this design to come up with these findings even if every person who died in a house fire was a non‐smoker. This flaw is sometimes called the ecological fallacy and is a consequence of the use of aggregated data rather than the more usual research method of collecting data for each individual study participant. The other three types of analytic study set out below are more satisfactory approaches to cause‐and‐effect questions because they are able to relate the supposed risk factor directly to the outcome in each study participant.

      Some cross‐sectional studies aim to shed light on cause and effect by recording whether people with a disease were more likely than people without the same disease to have experienced exposure to a risk factor. For example, for more than half a century researchers have recognized that patients with schizophrenia, when compared with the general population, are more likely to come from lower socio‐economic classes. What is a lot less clear is whether lower socio‐economic status is a risk factor for schizophrenia or, conversely, whether schizophrenia causes a slide down the socio‐economic scale. The cross‐sectional study design – in which the researcher measures in each subject a supposed risk factor at the same time as recording the presence of a condition – will nearly always have this chicken or egg problem (which comes first?).

      Source: From Salmon et al. (1998), © 1998, BMJ Publishing Group Ltd.

      It is a limitation of cross‐sectional designs that the direction of any effect cannot be determined because the supposed risk factor and the outcome are identified at the same time. The next two analytic study designs tackle this weakness and are able to identify the direction of any effect.

      A more satisfactory way of investigating cause and effect is to concentrate on a clinical scenario in which the characteristic that you suspect might be a risk factor and the outcome can only have arisen in that order. Consider for a moment smoking and lung cancer: it is plain that contracting lung cancer cannot have led someone to become a long‐standing heavy smoker.

      Case–control study is the label applied to a study such as the one just mentioned, about high blood pressure and stroke. The group of people with the condition are called the cases and they are compared with another group who are free of the disease and are called the controls. The comparison to be drawn is the exposure of each of the two groups to a supposed risk factor: were the cases more often exposed to the risk than were the controls? For further discussion of cases and controls, see Chapters 16 and 17.

An illustration of a case–control study examining the relation between cannabis use and onset of psychosis.

      Source: From Di Forti et al. (2019), © 2019, Elsevier.

      Another way of identifying any relation

Скачать книгу