The Concise Encyclopedia of Applied Linguistics. Carol A. Chapelle

Чтение книги онлайн.

Читать онлайн книгу The Concise Encyclopedia of Applied Linguistics - Carol A. Chapelle страница 54

The Concise Encyclopedia of Applied Linguistics - Carol A. Chapelle

Скачать книгу

analysis). Rather, dialogues are presented in the current entry by looking at three interdependent aspects of spoken communication: organization, intersubjectivity, and social context. Organization encompasses the mechanics of spoken interaction (e.g., turn taking); intersubjectivity is related to how interactants jointly construct meaning; and social context deals with the sociocultural issues, including historical factors, that shape dialogues. Each aspect of a dialogue will be discussed in this order.

      1 (1)Speaker A:How old are you?(Question)Speaker B:I'm twenty‐nine(Answer)

      The question in the above dialogue represents the first pair part of the adjacency pair, while the answer is the second pair part. It should be noted, however, that the term “adjacency pair” is slightly misleading, as first and second pair parts need not be adjacently placed.

      1 (2)Speaker A:How old are you?(Question 1)Speaker B:How old are YOU?(Question 2)Speaker A:Thirty‐two(Answer 2)Speaker B:Twenty‐nine(Answer 1)

      Examples 1 and 2 demonstrate that adjacency pairs play an important organizational role in co‐constructing meaning in dialogues. By taking a turn at talk, a speaker creates a set of dialogic conditions for the next turn, but at the same time reveals how the previous turn has been interpreted. In other words, any given turn in a dialogue is potentially conditionally relevant to preceding and subsequent turns. The basic organizational principle of conditional relevance is a defining feature of dialogues, as discussed using different theoretical constructs by scholars working in disciplines ranging from multimodal discourse analysis to pragmatics.

      Turn taking is also organized according to how one speaker makes the transition to the next. Turn transitions are accomplished in two ways: current speaker selects next speaker or next speaker nominates self (Sacks et al., 1974). Current speaker can select next speaker by, for example, asking a question (see examples 1 and 2), while self‐nomination requires the next speaker to interject with a word or utterance. Most research done on turn transitions adopt the term “transition relevant place” (TRP) to understand how dialogues are organized (Sacks et al., 1974). In dialogues, turn transitions often occur near, but not necessarily at the end of, completed turns (Schegloff, 2000). In other words, overlapping talk is a relatively common phenomenon in dialogues. This is because dialogues unfold, from one turn to another, when the next speaker is provided with enough information in the prior turn to communicate; take, for example, the following transition in the next excerpt.

      1 (3)Speaker A:Would you [like to join me?Speaker B:[Yes, I would love to.

      In the above dialogue, as indicated by the two open brackets, speaker B anticipates the question and responds mid‐utterance to speaker A's invitation. The answer occurs before speaker A is able to complete her question because dialogues are organized according to not only completed turns, but also the meaning that is co‐constructed within and between utterances. The organization of dialogues, including when TRPs occur, reveal much about the interactants and interactional context. For instance, analyzing the organization of dialogues may uncover the ways in which identities are negotiated through discourse and how such critical issues shape participation in human encounters (Stamou, 2018).

      A conversational floor (or simply floor) is “a collaborative venture where several people” operate “on the same wavelength” (Edelsky, 1981, p. 383). The notion of communicating on the same discursive wavelength suggests that a dialogue is much more than a series of turns or adjacency pairs; from the perspective of a (conversational) floor, a dialogue is a discursive space where appropriate forms and ways of communicating are negotiated in situ, and according to the larger social context in which interactants find themselves communicating. Specifically, a dialogue is a floor that reflects social norms and is made up of a series of turns; dialogue participants contribute to a floor by determining what topic to discuss, establishing participatory roles, and negotiating larger social issues, such as an agenda for a business meeting. A floor can be characterized as having a thematic or functional direction (Jenks, 2007). For example, a floor can be related to the talk of negotiating a business deal or the teaching of a lesson. Examining dialogues at the floor level has revealed that a turn can be collaboratively constructed, and, despite the work of conversation analysts, the direction of a floor need not develop in a linear, one‐speaker‐per‐turn fashion (Hayashi, 1991). In fact, in multiparty dialogues— especially in situations where four or more interactants are present—it is not uncommon for one conversational floor to morph into two or more. This transformational phenomenon is often referred to as a conversational schism (Egbert, 1997). In conversational schisms, many interactants take turns at the same time while contributing to different floors, which is common in many computer‐mediated dialogues (Herring, 1999; Jenks, 2014).

      Understanding dialogues also requires uncovering the ways in which intersubjectivity is achieved within a floor or across a series of turns. That is, how do interactants make sense of each other's turns and floor contributions? Achieving intersubjectivity—or, in simple terms, to co‐construct meaning—is accomplished in a number of ways. The current section will examine two issues related to understanding the process in which interactants achieve intersubjectivity: resolving communicative troubles and maintaining understanding in the face of ostensibly no trouble in communication.

      Understanding the first issue—that is, the ways in which interactants resolve troubles in dialogues—requires identifying where the trouble source is. A trouble is an umbrella term that covers all problems, mistakes, slips of the tongue, tips of the tongue, and mishearings that may occur in dialogues. For example, in example 4, speaker B requests clarification and, as a result, treats speaker A's previous turn as the source of trouble.

      1 (4)Speaker A:I goed to the zoo.Speaker B:Huh?Speaker A:I went to the zoo.

Скачать книгу