The Concise Encyclopedia of Applied Linguistics. Carol A. Chapelle

Чтение книги онлайн.

Читать онлайн книгу The Concise Encyclopedia of Applied Linguistics - Carol A. Chapelle страница 58

The Concise Encyclopedia of Applied Linguistics - Carol A. Chapelle

Скачать книгу

and to index a cool, nonchalant stance all the while (see Bucholtz, 2009, p. 164).

      Other scholars, such as Hall and Bucholtz (1995, pp. 183–4), link gendered language to different factors such as the speaker's agency, age, educational background, and ethnicity, and—to the perennial fascination of college students—have even interpreted meanings of gendered language through the prism of the phone sex industry in San Francisco. Hall argues that:

      This high‐tech mode of linguistic exchange complicates traditional notions of power in language, because the women working within the industry consciously produce a language stereotypically associated with women's powerlessness in order to gain economic power and social flexibility.

      She further argues that:

      The very existence of the term sweet talk—an activity that, in the American heterosexual mainstream, has become associated more with the speech patterns of women than those of men—underscores the ideological connection between women's language and sexual language.

      In response to economic incentives and requests to reinforce stereotypical gendered images, phone sex workers (both men and women) are exploiting gendered language for their own economic survival. They come from all walks of life, different ethnicities and age groups, and are trying to make a living (and make fun) of the gendered language. They do not naively or passively speak in a language reflecting their subordinate status. Instead, they actively seek out ways to profit from their clientele's lack of imagination by exploiting gendered language.

      We will end this section with an example of men adopting women's language for strategic purposes. As Holmes (1992, p. 317) observes, researchers recorded the speech of witnesses in a law court and found that male witnesses used more women's language features than women witnesses with more expertise in court or a higher occupational status. The following example illustrates this.

      Lawyer: And you saw, you observed what?

      Witness C: Well, after I heard—I can't really, I can't definitely state whether the brakes or the lights came first, but I rotated my head slightly to the right, and looked directly behind Mr. Z, and I saw reflections of lights, and uh, very very instantaneously after that I heard a very, very loud explosion—from my standpoint of view it would have been an implosion because everything was forced outward like this, like a grenade thrown into the room. And uh, it was, it was terrifically loud.

      In this exchange, the male witness used “women's” language—hedges and boosters—in his account of what happened. Sex (i.e., being a man or a woman) was not a determining factor for this kind of gendered language use. Instead, it was used to enact the “powerless” role of the witness (who is not a woman in this case) in his interaction with the lawyer or in his recounting of what had happened, and may even have other pragmatic effects, such as avoiding accountability or responsibilities and thus expecting leniency or acquittal.

      To conclude, gender is a complex social concept embedded in our biological wiring and socialization, as well as in mundane and professional interactions with others. Language can either reflect and reinforce a conventional gendered relationship or subvert stereotypical gendered images, as we see in the sweet talk performed by both men and women in high‐tech phone sex industries in San Francisco, or in men's adoption of women's language for courtroom interactions for strategic purposes.

      SEE ALSO: Analysis of Dialogue; Language and Identity

      1 Brownell, S., & Wasserstrom, J. (Eds.). (2002). Chinese femininities, Chinese masculinities: A reader. Berkeley: University of California Press.

      2 Bucholtz, M. (2009). Gender, interaction, and indexicality in Mexican immigrant youth slang. In A. Jaffe (Ed.), Stance: A sociolinguistic perspective (pp. 146–70). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

      3 Cameron, D., & Kulick, D. (2003). Language and sexuality. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

      4 Eckert, P., & McConnell, S. (1992). Think practically and look locally: Language and gender as community practice. Annual Review of Anthropology, 21, 461–90.

      5 Eckert, P., & McConnell, S. (Eds.). (2003). Language and gender. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

      6 Gal, S. (1991). Between speech and silence: The problematics of research on language and gender. In M. di Leonardo (Ed.), Gender at the crossroads of knowledge: Feminist anthropology in the postmodern era (pp. 175–203). Berkeley: University of California Press.

      7 Hall, K., & Bucholtz, M. (Eds.). (1995). Gender articulated: Language and the socially constructed self. London, England: Routledge.

      8 Holmes, J. (1992). An introduction to sociolinguistics. London, England: Longman.

      9 Jaffe, A. (2009). Introduction: The sociolinguistics of stance. In A. Jaffe (Ed.), Stance: A sociolinguistic perspective (pp. 3–25). Oxford, England: Oxford University Press.

      10 Keenan, E. (1974). Norm makers, norm breakers: Uses of speech by men and women in Malagasy community. In R. Brauman & J. Sherzer (Eds.), Explorations in the ethnography of speaking (pp. 125–44). Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press.

      11 Lakoff, R. (1990). Talking power: The politics of language. New York, NY: Basic Books.

      12 Newman, M. L., Groom, C. J., Handelman, L. D., & Peenebaker, J. W. (2008). Gender differences in language use: An analysis of 14,000 text samples. Discourse Processes, 45, 211–36.

      13 Tannen, D. (1990). You just don't understand: Women and men in conversation. New York, NY: William Morrow.

      14 Tannen, D. (1994). Gender and discourse. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

      1 Baron, B., & Kotthoff, H. (Eds.). (2001). Gender in interaction: Perspectives on femininity and masculinity in ethnography and discourse. Amsterdam, Netherlands: John Benjamins.

      2 Charteris‐Black, J. (2012). Shattering the bell jar: Metaphor, gender, and depression. Metaphor and Symbol, 27(3), 199–216.

      3 Coates, J. (2004). Women, men, and language: A sociolinguistic account of gender differences in language. Harlow, England: Longman.

      4 Crawford, M. (1995). Talking difference: On gender and language. London, England: Sage.

      5 Duranti, A., & Goodwin, C. (Eds.). (1992). Rethinking context: Language as an interactive phenomenon. New

Скачать книгу