Student Engagement Techniques. Elizabeth F. Barkley

Чтение книги онлайн.

Читать онлайн книгу Student Engagement Techniques - Elizabeth F. Barkley страница 20

Student Engagement Techniques - Elizabeth F.  Barkley

Скачать книгу

for each individual. (p. 8)

      The value of a well-developed schema is revealed in research on the differences between the learning of novices and experts. For the expert in any subject, new information is quickly grasped in useable form because connections to existing knowledge are numerous. The learning of a novice, in contrast, is labored and slow, not because the novice is less intelligent than the expert but because connections between new information and existing schemata are sparse—there are no hooks on which to hang the new information, no way to organize it (Cross, 1999, p. 8; de Groot, 1966).

      There are many different devices or behaviors that can help learners acquire and integrate new information with existing knowledge as well as retrieve stored information. These strategies include previewing, summarizing, paraphrasing, imaging, creating analogies, note taking, and outlining. Most experienced learners use strategies such as these to keep their attention focused on the task and their minds actively engaged. These strategies are not necessarily known or used by novice learners. We know, for example, that for new learning to take place, it has to be related to what the learner already knows. The challenge for some students—particularly underachieving students—is that existing knowledge is poorly organized and distressingly sparse. Cross (1993c) offers the analogy of a clothes closet. It is rather easy to hang clothes in a well-organized closet and retrieve them in usable form. Shoes, whether running shoes or dress shoes, have something in common and go on the floor; blouses and shirts are short and can use abbreviated hanging space; some things, such as slacks, go on special hangers; while other items such as sweaters and knits are probably best folded and placed on shelves. The point is that adding and retrieving items is easy when you understand and implement the organizing principles of the closet. If, on the other hand, you just throw things into the closet every which way (cramming it in and shutting the door quickly in the hopes that nothing will spill out!), then it will be a challenge to find the shirt you are looking for, or you might find only one of the socks.

      Transfer

      When activating prior learning to make sense of something new, the brain searches for any past learnings that are similar to, or associated with, the new learning. If the experiences exist, the corresponding neuronal networks or schema are activated, reinforcing the already-stored information as well as assisting in interpreting and assigning meaning to the new information. Svinicki (2004) notes that there are many types of transfer discussed in the literature, but two types are the most important for purposes of instruction (p. 99). The first is positive versus negative transfer. If the connections are accurate, the search results in “positive” transfer that can aid the learner in understanding and integrating new learnings. If, on the other hand, the connections are incorrect, the result is negative transfer, which creates confusion and errors. For example, when teaching Romance languages to English speakers, teachers frequently encounter positive transfer (e.g., “mucho” in Spanish sounds similar to “much” in English) and negative transfer (“librairie” in French sounds like “library,” but means “bookstore”) (Sousa, 2006, pp. 138–139).

      The second type of transfer is near versus far transfer. This distinction refers to the type of task: near transfer tasks are those that look very much alike and follow the same rules for responding, while a far transfer task is where the same rules apply, but they are transferred to a different setting. “Far transfer” requires more thinking on the part of the learner. Svinicki (2004) offers driving a midsize automatic sedan as an example: if you've driven one, you can easily drive any other because the steering wheel, gear shift, windshield wipers, and turn signals all look alike and are in the same position. If, on the other hand, you get into a car that is very different (such as a convertible, stick-shift sports car), your normal driving responses are not instantly triggered and you have to stop and figure out where everything is. The rules are the same, but the car looks different. Moving between different midsize automatic sedans is a near transfer task; moving from a mid-level automatic sedan to a stick-shift sports car is a far transfer task (Svinicki, 2004, pp. 100–101). There are several factors that affect the quality of transfer: similarity/difference, association, and context and degree of original learning.

      Similarity and Differences

      How similar a previously encountered situation is to a new situation affects transfer. Interestingly, it appears that the brain generally stores new information in networks that contain similar characteristics or associations, but it retrieves information by identifying how the information is different from the other items in that network. For example, the visual appearances of people we know seem to be stored in the network of what humans look like (e.g., torso, head, two arms, two legs), but if we are trying to find someone we know in a crowd, we will look for the characteristics that distinguish them from other people in the group (e.g., facial characteristics, height, voice, and so forth). Obviously when there is high similarity with few differences, distinguishing between the two becomes more difficult (Sousa, 2006, p. 143). Thus, the potential for negative transfer is higher when concepts, principles, and data, or the labels for this information, are similar. For example, in music, “whole tone” and “whole note” sound similar, but the terms represent very different concepts (whole tone is a specific distance between two pitches, while whole note is the rhythmic duration of a single pitch).

      Association

      Learning two items together such that the two are bonded or associated also affects transfer, and when one of the items is recalled, the other is spontaneously recalled as well. When we hear or read “Romeo,” we unconsciously add “and Juliet,” or when we think of trademark symbols such as McDonald's golden arches or Apple's apple logo, we immediately think of the associated product (Sousa, 2006, p. 145). Since everything we know and understand is preserved as a network of associations, the more associations we make, the greater the number of potential places we have to attach new information and the easier it is for us to learn and retain that information. In short, the more we learn and retain, the more we can learn and retain.

      Context and Degree of Original Learning

      Not

Скачать книгу