Engineering Acoustics. Malcolm J. Crocker

Чтение книги онлайн.

Читать онлайн книгу Engineering Acoustics - Malcolm J. Crocker страница 61

Engineering Acoustics - Malcolm J. Crocker

Скачать книгу

oval window causes pressure waves in the fluid of the upper gallery of the cochlea (Figures 4.3 and 4.4). The fluid in the lower gallery is separated from that in the upper gallery by the cochlear duct containing the organ of Corti. The organ has about 35 000 sensitive hair cells distributed along its length which are connected in a complicated way to about 18 000 nerve fibers which are combined into the auditory nerve which runs into the brain. The pressure waves cause the basilar membrane to deflect and a shearing motion occurs between the basilar and tectorial membranes. The hair cells sense the shearing motion and if the stimulus is great enough the neuron to which each hair cell is attached sends an impulse along the nerve fiber to the brain cortex [8]. Each neuron takes about 1/1000th of a second to recharge and so individual neurons are limited to “firing” no more than 1000 times/second. With the neurons, a triggering level must be reached before they “fire” and so they have an all‐or‐nothing response. The brain must interpret the neural impulses to give us the sensation of hearing and, as we can imagine, the way in which this is done is not well understood.

      4.2.3 Theories of Hearing

      In 1543, Andreas Vesalius published his treatise on anatomy giving a description of the middle ear and in 1561 Gabriello Fallopio described the cochlea itself.

      In 1605 Gaspard Bauhin put forward a resonance theory for the ear. In his model, different air cavities were excited by different frequencies. However, he knew little of the construction of the inner ear. Du Verney, in 1633, developed a more advanced theory by postulating that different parts of the ridge of bone which twists up the inside of the cochlea resonated at different frequencies which depended upon its width. Du Verney's theory was held until 1851 when Alfonso Corti, using a microscope, discovered that the thousands of hair cells on the basilar membrane were attached to the ridge of bone in the cochlea.

      A few years later, Hermann von Helmholtz used Corti's findings to suggest a new theory of hearing. In Helmholtz's theory, as it became refined, different parts of the basilar membrane resonated at different frequencies. Later workers showed that Helmholtz was not exactly right (the basilar membrane is not under tension). However, in 1928 Georg von Békésy did show that waves do travel along the basilar membrane and different sections of the basilar membrane do respond more than others to a certain sound. The region of maximum response is frequency‐dependent and as Helmholtz had predicted, von Békésy found that the high‐frequency sound is detected nearer to the oval window and the low‐frequency sound, nearer to the apex (Figures 4.3 and 4.4).

      So far we have traced the sound signal down the ear canal to the eardrum, through the auditory ossicles, through the oval window to the cochlear fluid to the basilar membrane and the hair cells, and finally to the neural impulses sent to the brain. How does the brain interpret these signals? Our study now enters the realm of psychology. While the physicist or engineer talks about sound pressure level and frequency, the psychologist talks about loudness and pitch, respectively. The human auditory response to sound is studied by psychoacoustics. In Section 4.3 we shall discuss the relationships between some of the engineering descriptions of sound and the psychological or subjective descriptions of psychoacoustics.

      4.3.1 Hearing Envelope

Schematic illustration of human auditory field envelope.

      4.3.2 Loudness Measurement

      The way in which the brain interprets the neural pulses is still a matter for research. However, various experiments have been conducted on groups of people to determine people's average sensation of loudness, etc. We should stress that no one's hearing is exactly the same as any other and hence we must find statistical responses.

Schematic illustration of equal loudness contours in which the contours join the sound pressure levels of different frequency pure tones that are judged to be equally loud. The numbers on each contour are the loudness levels in phons.

Скачать книгу