Population Genetics. Matthew B. Hamilton

Чтение книги онлайн.

Читать онлайн книгу Population Genetics - Matthew B. Hamilton страница 16

Population Genetics - Matthew B. Hamilton

Скачать книгу

them with simulation results.

      For a history of population genetics from Darwin to the 1930s, see:

      1 Provine, W.B. (1971). The Origins of Theoretical Population Genetics. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.For a concise history of population genetics since the mid‐1960s that highlights major conceptual advances as well as technical innovations to measure genetic variation, see:

      2 Charlesworth, B. and Charlesworth, D. (2017). Population genetics from 1966 to 2016. Heredity 118: 2–9.For two personal and historical essays on the past, present, and assumptions of theoretical population genetics, see:

      3 Lewontin, R.C. (1985). Population genetics. In: Evolution: Essays in Honour of John Maynard Smith (eds. P.J. Greenwood, P.H. Harvey and M. Slatkin), 3–18. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

      4 Wakeley, J. (2005). The limits of theoretical population genetics. Genetics 169: 1–7.

      2.1 Mendel's model of particulate genetics

       Mendel's breeding experiments.

       Independent assortment of alleles.

       Independent segregation of loci.

       Some common genetic terminology.

      From 1856 to 1863, the Augustinian monk Gregor Mendel carried out experiments with pea plants that demonstrated the concept of particulate inheritance. Mendel showed that phenotypes are determined by discrete units that are inherited intact and unchanged through generations. His hypothesis was sufficient to explain three common observations: (i) phenotype is sometimes identical between parents and offspring; (ii) offspring phenotype can differ from that of the parents; and (iii) “pure” phenotypes of earlier generations could skip generations and reappear in later generations. Neither blending inheritance nor inheritance of acquired characteristics are satisfactory explanations for all of these observations. It is hard for us to fully appreciate now, but Mendel's results were truly revolutionary and served as the very foundation of population genetics. The lack of an accurate mechanistic model of heredity severely constrained biological explanations of cause and effect up to the point that Mendel's results were “rediscovered” in the year 1900.

      It is worthwhile to briefly review the experiments with pea plants that Mendel used to demonstrate independent assortment of both alleles within a locus and of multiple loci, sometimes dubbed Mendel's first and second laws. We need to remember that this was well before the Punnett square, which originated in about 1905. Therefore, the conceptual tool we would use now to predict progeny genotypes from parental genotypes was a thing of the future. So, in revisiting Mendel's experiments, we will not use the Punnett square in an attempt to follow his logic. Mendel only observed the phenotypes of generations of pea plants that he had hand‐pollinated. From these phenotypes and their patterns of inheritance, he inferred the existence of heritable factors. His experiments were actually both logical and clever, but are now taken for granted since the basic mechanism of particulate inheritance has long since ceased to be an open question. It was Mendel who established the first and most fundamental prediction of population genetics: expected genotype frequencies.

Schematic illustration of Mendel self-pollinated the F2 progeny produced by the cross shown in Figure 2.2. Of the F2 progeny that had a phenotype, 1/3 produced all progeny with a yellow phenotype and 2/3 produced progeny with a 3 : 1 ratio of yellow and green progeny.

Скачать книгу