The Handbook of Speech Perception. Группа авторов

Чтение книги онлайн.

Читать онлайн книгу The Handbook of Speech Perception - Группа авторов страница 52

The Handbook of Speech Perception - Группа авторов

Скачать книгу

taste like?” or even “What does a society look like?” are difficult to answer, because societies are not the kinds of things that we taste or see. Societies are not like strawberries. But even abstract words like ‘society’ may contain embodied semantics that become apparent when we consider the ways in which metaphors link abstract concepts with concretely experienced objects (Lakoff & Johnson, 1980). One feature of societies, we might assert, is that they have insides and outsides. In this respect, they are like a great many objects that we experience directly: cups, bowls and rooms. Therefore, it may be hypothesized that even abstract words such as ‘society’ could have predictable effects on the sensorimotor system. Brain areas such as the insula that respond to the physical disgust of fetid smells also respond to the social disgust of seeing an appalled look on someone else’s face (Wicker et al., 2003). There are limits, however, to the embodied view of meaning. Function words such as conjunctions and prepositions are more difficult to associate with concrete experiences. As we have described it, the approach is also limited to finding meaning in the sensorimotor systems, which is unsatisfying as it ignores large swathes of the brain. In the next subsection, we turn to a more ambitious, if abstract, way of mapping the meaning of words that is not limited to finding meaning in the sensorimotor systems.

       Vector representations and encoding models

      One difficulty in studying meaning is that “meaning” can be challenging to define. If you ask what the word ‘strawberry’ means, we might point at a strawberry. If we know the activity in your visual system that is triggered by looking at a strawberry, then we can point to similar activity patterns in your visual system when you think of the word ‘strawberry’ as another kind of meaning. You might imagine that it is harder to point to just any part of the brain and ask of its current state, “Is this a representation of ‘strawberry’?” But it is not impossible. In this subsection, we will, in as informal a way as possible, introduce the ideas of vector representations of words, and encoding models for identifying the neural representations of vectors.

      Generally speaking, an encoding model aims to predict how the brain will respond to a stimulus. Encoding models contrast with decoding models, which aim to do the opposite: guess which stimulus caused the brain response. The spectrogram reconstruction method (mentioned in a previous section) is an example of a decoding model (Mesgarani et al., 2008). An encoding model of sound would therefore try to predict the neural response to an audio recording. In a landmark study of semantic encoding, Mitchell et al. (2008) were able to predict fMRI responses to the meanings of concrete nouns, like ‘celery’ and ‘airplane.’ Unlike studies of embodied meaning, Mitchell et al. (2008) were able to predict neural responses that were not limited to the sensorimotor systems. For instance, they predicted accurate word‐specific neural responses across bilateral occipital and parietal lobes, the fusiform and middle frontal gyri, and sensory cortex; the left inferior frontal gyrus; the medial frontal gyrus and the anterior cingulate (see Figure 3.6 for reference; Mitchell et al., 2008). These encoding results expand the number of regions to which the meaning of a word might be distributed, to nonsensory systems like the anterior cingulate. An even greater expansion of these semantic regions can be found in more recent work (Huth et al., 2016).

      1 airplane

      2 boat

      3 celery

      4 strawberry

      One way to encode each of these as a list of numbers is to simply assign one number to each word: ‘airplane’ = [1], ‘boat’ = [2], ‘celery’ = [3], and ‘strawberry’ = [4]. We have enclosed the numbers in square brackets to mean that these are lists. Note that it is possible to have only one item in a list. A good thing about this encoding of the words, as lists of numbers, is that the resulting lists are short and easy to decode: we only have to look them up in our memory or in a table. But this encoding does not do a very good job of capturing the differences in meanings between the words. For example, ‘airplane’ and ‘boat’ are both manufactured vehicles that you could ride inside, whereas ‘celery’ and ‘strawberry’ are both edible parts of plants. A more involved semantic coding might make use of all of these descriptive features to produce the following representations.

Word Manufactured Vehicle Ride inside Edible Plant part
airplane 1 1 1 0 0
boat 1 1 1 0 0
celery 0 0 0 1

Скачать книгу