The Handbook of Speech Perception. Группа авторов

Чтение книги онлайн.

Читать онлайн книгу The Handbook of Speech Perception - Группа авторов страница 60

The Handbook of Speech Perception - Группа авторов

Скачать книгу

voice. Furthermore, during passive listening, neither perturbation produced any N100 amplitude differences. This suggests that suppression of the auditory cortex is greatest when afferent sensory feedback matches an expected outcome specifically during speech production (Heinks‐Maldonado et al., 2005).

      These studies and many others support the existence of neural mechanisms that use the auditory character of the talker’s speech output to control articulation. However, the challenge of mapping high‐level computational models to behavioral and neural data remains. The necessity of different levels of description and the units within the levels are difficult to determine. In short, while there may be only a single neural architecture that manages fluent speech, many abstract cognitive models could be consistent with this architecture (see Griffiths, Lieder, & Goodman, 2015, for a discussion of constraints on cognitive modeling). An additional approach is to examine ontogeny for relationships between perception and production.

      Much is made about the uniqueness of human language and at the speech level, the frequent focus of these uniqueness claims is on the perceptual skills of the developing infant. However, the less emphasized side of communication, speaking, is clearly a specialized behavior. Humans are part of a small cohort of species that are classified as vocal learners and who acquire the sounds in their adult repertoire through social learning (Petkov & Jarvis, 2012). This trait seems to be an example of convergent evolution in a few mammalian (humans, dolphins, whales, seal, sea lions, elephants, and bats) and bird (songbirds, parrots, and hummingbirds) species. The behavioral similarities shown by these disparate species are mirrored by their neuroanatomy and gene expression. In a triumph of behavioral, genetic, and neuroanatomic research, a consortium of scientists has shown similarities in brain pathways for vocal learners that are not observed in species that do not learn their vocal repertoires (Pfenning et al., 2014).

      The DIVA model proposes that early vocal development involves a closed‐loop imitation process driven initially by two stages of babbling and later a vocal learning stage that directly involves corrective feedback processing. In the first babbling phase, random articulatory motions generate somatosensory and acoustic consequences, and a mapping of the developing vocal tract as a speech device takes place. Separately, infants learn the perceptual categories of their native language. This is crucial to the model. As Guenther (1995, p. 599) states, “the model starts out with the ability to perceive all of the sounds that it will eventually learn to produce.” When the second stage of babbling begins, it involves the mapping between phonetic categories developed through perceptual learning and articulation. The babbling during this period tunes the feedback system to permit corrective responses to detected errors. In the next imitation phase, infants hear adult syllables that they try to reproduce. In a cyclical process involving sensory feedback from actual productions and better feedforward commands, the system shapes the early utterances toward the native language.

      Simulations by Guenther and his students support the logic of this account. However, there are significant concerns. First among these is that the data supporting this process are weak or, in the case of MacDonald et al.’s (2012) results, contradict the hypothesis. Early speech feedback processing and the shaping of speech production targets is not well attested. Second, the proposal relies on a strong relationship between the representations of speech perception and speech production. Surprisingly, this relationship is controversial.

      Models like DIVA predict a phenomenon that is frequently assumed but is not strongly supported by actual data – babbling drift. The hypothesis that the sounds of babbling drift over time was first proposed by Roger Brown (1958). Brown suggested that the phonetic repertoire in the babbling of infants slowly begins to resemble the phonetics of the language environment that they are exposed to and begins to not include sounds that are absent from the native language. As the review by Best et al. (2016) indicates, the support for this idea is mixed, particularly from transcription studies and perceptual studies where naive listeners attempted to identify the language environment of the infant’s babbling.

Schematic illustration of average F1 (circles) and F2 (triangles) frequencies estimates across time for adults (top panel), young children (middle panel), and toddlers (bottom panel).

      Source: MacDonald et al., © 2012, Elsevier.

      Another approach has been to use recordings of infants babbling as perceptual stimuli and ask adult listeners to categorize what native language the infants have. These studies have also shown mixed results, with some

Скачать книгу