The Handbook of Language and Speech Disorders. Группа авторов

Чтение книги онлайн.

Читать онлайн книгу The Handbook of Language and Speech Disorders - Группа авторов страница 56

The Handbook of Language and Speech Disorders - Группа авторов

Скачать книгу

controls, but not on regular forms (Clahsen & Almazan, 1998; Penke & Krause, 2004; Zukowski, 2004). However, no study to date has reported that individuals with WS outperform mental‐age controls, either on regulars or irregulars. Furthermore, the results of studies that have employed a larger number of participants (such as Thomas et al., 2001) show no interaction between group and regularity. As pointed out by Brock (2007), all studies that have investigated the performance of individuals with WS on regular versus irregular inflections suffer from “ceiling” effects, in that most of the participants in all the studies perform at ceiling on regulars, which makes it impossible for any group differences on irregulars to be found.

      Some evidence suggests that the older the individuals with WS, the better their language abilities. The seminal studies by Bellugi and colleagues (Bellugi et al., 1988, 1994) had adolescents in their studies. Jarrold, Baddeley, and Hewes (1998) also showed that the verbal advantage in individuals with WS becomes more prominent as they get older. More recent studies, such as Musolino, Chunyo, and Landau (2010), which argue that individuals with WS have acquired the same core aspects of the computational component of language as neurotypical individuals, also report data from adolescents and adults with WS.

      There have been fewer studies of infants and toddlers with WS compared with ones on children, adolescents and adults, and although informative, the evidence is inconclusive. There is evidence for an asynchronous relationship between language and other cognitive abilities due to poorer language than cognitive abilities (Stojanovik & James, 2006), but also due to stronger language than cognitive abilities (Mervis & Bertrand, 1997). It is generally agreed, though, that the onset of language acquisition in WS in the early stages is delayed (Paterson, Girelli, Gsodl, Johnson, & Karmiloff‐Smith, 1999; Semel & Rosner, 2003; Stojanovik & James, 2006). The median age at which infants with WS have a vocabulary of 10 expressive words is 28 months, compared with 13.5 months in neurotypical infants (Mervis, Robinson, Rowe, Becerra, & Klein‐Tasman, 2003). Based on longitudinal data from Hebrew‐speaking children with WS, Levy and Eilam (2013) reported that infants with WS started to use combinatorial language on average 24 months later than their typically developing peers.

      The reasons for the early language delay are still unknown. A recent study of statistical learning of young infants with WS aged between 8 and 20 months showed that they were able to discriminate between statistically defined “words” and “part words” in an AGL paradigm (Cashon, Ha, Estes, Saffran, & Mervis, 2016). This shows that infants with WS were able to detect statistical regularities in the speech stream, which suggests that the delayed early lexical acquisition of infants with WS may not be the result of their inability to segment words from the speech stream.

      Early language acquisition in WS is generally an under‐researched area, and more longitudinal studies are needed in order to find out how language is acquired in WS and the relationship between language and other cognitive abilities.

      Despite a large body of research into the morpho‐syntactic abilities of people with WS, there have been fewer studies investigating actual communication or pragmatic skills in this population. There have been reports that good social communication skills are a “hallmark” of the syndrome (Jones et al., 2000). Mervis, Klein‐Tasman, and Mastin (2001), using the Vineland Adaptive Behaviour Scales (VABS; Sparrow, Balla, & Cichetti, 1984), reported that communication in WS is a relative strength. In particular, Jones et al. (2000) argued that superior social‐communication skills distinguish this population from populations with other developmental disorders, such as DS and autism. In a series of tasks, Jones et al. (2000) reported that children with WS include a higher number of inferences about the affective state and motivation of story characters in comparison to typically developing children and children with DS. In the same study, individuals with WS provided a greater number of descriptions of affective states and evaluative comments during an interview task, and were more likely to ask questions of the interviewer. This was interpreted as showing that individuals with WS are “hypersocial.”

      Some studies have shown that individuals with WS are not sensitive to the needs of the conversational partner (Udwin & Yule, 1991). Further to this, Stojanovik et al. (2001) reported a high level of conversational inadequacy in a pilot study of a group of children with WS, in which participants with WS were found to have a tendency not to provide enough information for the conversational partner. Laws and Bishop (2004) reported pragmatic language impairment and social deficits in a group of older children and young adults with WS, using the Children’s Communication Checklist (Bishop, 1998). The checklist ratings showed pragmatic language deficits, evident from inappropriate initiations of

Скачать книгу