The Handbook of Language and Speech Disorders. Группа авторов

Чтение книги онлайн.

Читать онлайн книгу The Handbook of Language and Speech Disorders - Группа авторов страница 57

The Handbook of Language and Speech Disorders - Группа авторов

Скачать книгу

Stojanovik (2006) reported that children with WS have difficulties with exchange structure and responding appropriately to the interlocutor’s requests for information and clarification. They also had significant difficulties with interpreting meaning and providing enough information for the conversational partner. A later, longitudinal study by John, Dobson, Thomas, and Mervis (2012) reported similar findings, in that they found a relationship between children’s pragmatic ability at 9–12 years of age and their earlier secondary subjectivity (i.e., the ability to produce utterances which are paired with eye‐contact).

      Lacroix, Bernicot, and Reilly (2007) investigated the abilities of children with WS to interact for the purpose of attaining a goal. The task required the mother and the child to collaborate and negotiate in order to produce a drawing on the computer, on the basis of a drawing model. During this collaborative conversation task, the children and adolescents with WS (similarly to those with DS) produced fewer utterances than the typically developing participants and played a weak role in the conversation compared to their mother. However, they readily expressed their psychological states (like younger children of the same nonverbal mental age). Also, the children and adolescents with WS responded to maternal directives less often than all other groups.

      Children with WS also find it difficult to verbalize message inadequacy (John, Rowe, & Mervis, 2009). Using a barrier, listener‐role referential communication task, the child was instructed to follow the researcher’s instructions. The children performed well when the instructions were adequate, but found it considerably more difficult when the instructions were inadequate, that is, when the instructions were ambiguous, or the instruction contained vocabulary the child did not understand, in that they indicated less than half of the time that there was a problem. When they verbalized that there was a problem, the verbalizations were often too vague for the researcher to identify a problem, or they identified the wrong problem. The ability to verbalize message inadequacy was related to the children’s chronological age and theory of mind.

      In summary, although it is evident that research on WS has moved substantially from the initial claims that individuals with WS have “intact” language abilities despite severe cognitive deficits, the emerging picture is much less clear and the question of the contribution of atypical populations, such as individuals with WS to which constraints guide typical language acquisition, is more open to debate than ever before. The evidence base is growing, however. in the direction of highlighting possible atypical developmental pathways and relative strengths and weaknesses.

      DS is a genetic disorder caused by a partial or complete duplication of chromosome 21 (Epstein, 1986). It affects approximately 1 in 691 live births (Parker et al., 2010). DS is the most common cause of intellectual disability and seems to be particularly detrimental to language acquisition. A lot can be learnt about language in individuals with DS by reading reports on individuals with WS because people with DS are often included as a comparison group in studies of individuals with WS—individuals with WS and those with DS have similar nonverbal abilities but often different language profiles (Bellugi, Linchtenberger, Lai, & St.George, 2000; Bellugi et al., 1994; Jarrold et al., 1998; Reilly, Klima, & Bellugi, 1990).

      As in individuals with WS, the profile of individuals with DS is also often characterized as “uneven” with weaknesses in auditory short‐term memory relative to visual short‐term memory and other aspects of cognition (Chapman, 2003), and with strengths in social functioning abilities (Kasari & Baunminger, 1998).

      Only a handful of studies have investigated early language development in DS, hence knowledge on the early stages of language acquisition in DS is still limited. Cardoso‐Martins, Mervis, and Mervis (1985) followed longitudinally 6 children with DS aged 17–19 months at the start of the study, and compared them with 6 typically developing children aged 9 months at the start of the study, investigating the acquisition of object names. There was no difference between the two groups at the start, but the DS group (both comprehension and production) started to lag behind their general nonverbal cognitive skills. This suggests that vocabulary acquisition develops at a slower pace than general cognitive abilities from an early age. Similar findings were reported almost 30 years later by Zampini and D’Odorico (2013) who used the Italian version of the MacArthur–Bates Communicative Development Inventory with 18 children with DS aged between 2 and 3. Specifically, expressive vocabulary was reported to lag behind general cognitive development, with the main changes in vocabulary development occurring at 36 months of age, when individual differences become more prominent. However, different findings were reported in a longitudinal study of expressive vocabulary by Te Kaat‐van den Os, Volman, Jongmans, and Lauteslager (2017) who found that children’s expressive vocabulary skills were related to their nonverbal mental abilities. Similarly, a relationship between receptive vocabulary development and general nonverbal abilities was reported by Cuskelly, Povey, and Jobling (2016).

Скачать книгу