Second Language Pronunciation. Группа авторов

Чтение книги онлайн.

Читать онлайн книгу Second Language Pronunciation - Группа авторов страница 22

Second Language Pronunciation - Группа авторов

Скачать книгу

sounds or suprasegmentals.

      Instructed L2 Pronunciation Development

      While limits on ultimate attainment in naturalistic learning contexts are well-established, instructed L2 pronunciation can provide an opportunity to re-orient learners’ selective perception towards phonetic cues that they have learned to ignore. Despite this fact, production-oriented approaches to teaching pronunciation have long-dominated the field (e.g., Celce-Murcia et al., 2010; Lyster et al., 2013; Thomson & Derwing, 2015). In the 1970s, Audiolingualism used spoken models, but only to introduce production rehearsal activities (Brown & Lee, 2015). In the 1980s, Communicative Language Teaching (CLT) largely neglected pronunciation instruction, focusing almost exclusively on communicative processes to the exclusion of form. While CLT appealed to adult L2 learners’ desire to communicate as quickly as possible, it largely recreated the naturalistic conditions under which L2 pronunciation is most resistant to change. In more recent years, while pronunciation instruction has regained a position of importance, instruction continues to be predominantly production-focused (e.g., Saito & Lyster, 2012).

      Since we know that speech perception plays such a foundational role in accurate speech production, why is this not reflected in popular L2 pronunciation teaching methods? One reason for this disconnect is that L2 speech perception research is largely inaccessible to language teachers (Thomson, 2018a). It is typically published in technical journals and largely reports on laboratory-based studies, which may appear to lack relevance to the real world. Unfortunately, by ignoring this important research, pronunciation specialists are unprepared to teach in a way that addresses the cognitive-developmental features of pronunciation associated with speech perception (Derwing & Munro, 2015).

      The cognitive mechanisms underlying the benefits of HVPT over low variability training are not fully understood. One possibility is that all cognitive categories, by their very nature, contain variation. This means that learning a new category necessitates learning about the distribution of sounds that can occur within that category. Another possibility is that the use of multiple talkers maximizes the potential for a given L2 learner to encounter at least some tokens of target sounds that do not automatically assimilate to pre-existing L1 categories. There is evidence that L2 learners are more apt to recognize English vowels as belonging to a new category if those vowels were produced by a talker whose productions are acoustically distant from any confusable L1 vowel categories (Thomson, 2007). It remains unclear whether there is an optimal amount of variability. Programs claiming to be HVPT have used between 2 and 30 talkers, for example, and vary widely in the number of phonetic contexts utilized (Thomson, 2018a). It seems unlikely that using two talkers provides optimal variability, but using increasingly larger numbers may result in diminishing returns or make learning more difficult (Thomson, 2018a).

      Two other approaches to perceptual training are also worth noting, although neither has a sufficient evidence base to support its widespread adoption. Underlying both seems to be an assumption that accurate perception of one’s own productions (i.e., self-perception) has a facilitative effect in ultimately matching what one perceives with what one produces (Baker & Trofimovich, 2006; Borden et al., 1983). While neither is a purely perception-oriented technique, both include a perceptual component through the use of imitation. Rojczyk (2015) instructed Polish learners of English to imitate English-accented Polish sentences, targeting particular English consonants. This practice may have had the effect of orienting learners’ attention to phonetic information produced with their own voices in their L1. The researcher found that imitation of English-accented Polish led to positive transfer in their L2 English pronunciation. Other researchers are testing what they call a “Golden Speaker” approach to making perceptual learning easier (Ding et al., 2019). This is based on a belief that there are ideal voices from which particular learners can best develop L2 speech perception. The Golden Speaker web-based application maps a learner’s own voice quality onto the correct pronunciation of target sounds produced by a native speaker. The system then generates training stimuli that sound like the learner’s voice, but without segmental errors. It is assumed that this will make it easier for learners to attend to those parts of the acoustic signal that are distinct from their own voices, because it simulates a vocal tract size and shape that is exactly the same as the learners’ own, but with an articulation model that is native speaker-like. To the extent to which these alternative approaches to L2 perception training work, they may be preferable to HVPT, which is more labor-intensive since it requires the accumulation of training stimuli from multiple talkers.

      Pedagogical Implications

Скачать книгу